In the Shadows: A Review of the
Research on Plea Bargaining
Ram Subramanian, Léon Digard, Melvin Washington II, and Stephanie Sorage
September 2020
In the Shadows: A Review of the Research on Plea Bargaining iii
From the Director
When most people think of the American justice
system, they likely picture a courtroom with lawyers,
a judge, and a jury waiting to determine the facts of
the case and provide a just outcome. But the majori
of people “found guil” in America never stand trial:
their fate is determined in a courthouse hallway or
prosecutor’s office via a quick conversation between
aorneys.
Plea bargaining is more a part of the American justice
system than the formal trial and, in fact, makes up the
vast majori of criminal justice transactions today.
Only 2 percent of federal criminal cases—and a similar
number of state cases—are brought to trial. More than
90 percent of convictions, at both federal and state
levels, are the result of guil pleas. Plea bargaining is
so ndamental to the system that even in 1970, Chief
Justice Warren Burger of the U.S. Supreme Court
estimated that a 10 percent reduction in guil pleas
would require doubling the amount of judicial capaci
in the system. Scholars in recent years have suested
that the criminal legal system could be brought to a
halt by a mass resal to plead guil.
And yet lile is known about plea bargaining.
Pleas are offered and retracted at the unfeered
discretion of prosecutors. Bargains themselves are
undocumented and largely unchallenged, save for a
few formal questions meant to establish that the plea
is “voluntary, intelligent, and knowing.” To understand
plea bargaining, then, we must depend on a small but
growing body of research. rough interviews, data
gathered by courts, and other means, scholars are
aempting to understand the factors that influence
plea bargaining as well as whether a plea bargain is a
“bargain” at all.
ese studies are vital to understanding a process that
is so central to how the criminal legal system currently
operates, but they remain woelly inadequate and
incomplete. e sheer lack of trials, for example,
means that it is difficult to find analogous cases to
compare to those that end in pleas. In addition, the
dynamic and recursive nature of bargaining is difficult
to isolate into discrete questions for study. Finally,
the most important voices—the people subject to
these bargains—are largely absent from these studies.
Instead, data is provided by police, courts, prosecutors,
and jails. is one-sided understanding of the bargain
explains a good deal about how the legal characteristics
of a case, or pressures such as increasing caseloads,
drive prosecutorial bargaining, but it offers lile
insight into the reasons that people trade their right to
a trial for a faster and more certain conviction.
But why does it maer?
e history of the American justice system is a
history of mass incarceration, with wildly disparate
consequences for Black and white people. We are
faced with substantial evidence that people are put
in untenable positions aer arrest. ey are kept in
jails away from their families and communities if
they cannot afford cash bail or an aorney to argue
iv Vera Institute of Justice
for them. And although representation is guaranteed,
the tremendous caseload borne by public defenders
means that “representation” before trial is likely to be
pernctory and impersonal. Pretrial incarceration has
been definitively linked to the likelihood of conviction,
and most convictions are obtained via plea. And
although the Supreme Court has indicated that even
knowing you may be put to death if you face trial is
not “coercive” pressure to plead guil, it is difficult
to say that an incarcerated person faced with the loss
of their income, housing, family, and communi is
truly free to make a choice. Even aer people return
to their communities, a conviction—whether it stems
from a guil plea or a jury verdict—carries collateral
consequences that will follow them for years, if not the
rest of their lives.
Today, the American justice system is in crisis, but
it is also in a moment of unique opportuni. is
review and analysis of the available literature on plea
bargaining—released by Vera and the Safe and Justice
Challenge—represents decades of work by dozens
of scholars. It brings light to the shadowed hallways
where the majori of justice is transacted and new
aention to the ways the criminal legal system has
become an ad hoc administrative process.
Elizabeth Swavola
Acting Project Director
Vera Institute of Justice
About this report
is report is one of a series that the Vera Institute of Justice (Vera) is
releasing with the Safe and Justice Challenge—the John D. and Catherine
T. MacArthur Foundations initiative to reduce overincarceration by
changing the way America thinks about and uses jails. e initiative
is supporting a network of competitively selected local jurisdictions
commied to finding ways to safely reduce jail incarceration. Other
publications in the series to date include:
Incarcerations Front Door: e Misuse of Jails in America;
e Price of Jails: Measuring the Taxpayer Cost of Local Incarceration;
Overlooked: Women and Jails in an Era of Reform;
Out of Sight: e Growth of Jails in Rural America;
Divided Justice: Trends in Black and White Incarceration 1990-2013;
e New Dynamics of Mass Incarceration;
Justice Denied: e Harml and Lasting Effects of Pretrial Detention;
Gatekeepers: e Role of Police in Ending Mass Incarceration;
Broken Ground: Why America Keeps Building More Jails and What It Can
Do Instead; and
the multimedia storytelling project, e Human Toll of Jail.
rough the Safe and Justice Challenge, Veras offices in Los Angeles and
New Orleans, and direct partnerships with jurisdictions nationwide, Vera
is providing expert information and technical assistance to support local
efforts to stem the flow of people into jail, including using alternatives to
arrest and prosecution for minor offenses, recalibrating the use of bail,
and addressing fines and fees that trap people in jail. For more information
about Veras work to reduce the use of jails, contact Elizabeth Swavola,
acting project director, at eswavola@vera.org. For more information about
the Safe and Justice Challenge, visit .safeandjusticechallenge.org.
Contents
1 Introduction
10 Factors that influence plea bargaining
10 Coercive factors
19 Legal characteristics
24 Systemic inequities
31 The criminal law
35 Caseloads
39 Plea bargaining outcomes
40 How much of a bargain is a plea bargain?
44 Are innocent people induced to plead guilty?
48 Conclusion: Limitations and future directions
In the Shadows: A Review of the Research on Plea Bargaining 1
Introduction
T
he common narrative in popular culture is that criminal justice is
meted out in courtrooms around the country. Facts about a particular
case inevitably emerge from adversarial proceedings in which
prosecutors and defense aorneys go to bale in open court over maers
of fact and law, juries decide whether people are guil or not guil, and
judges determine appropriate punishments.
1
People have their days in
court, and the public—including victims, if there are any—witness whether
justice is done.
2
In fact, criminal trials are rare.
3
Instead, most criminal cases that result
in conviction—97 percent in large urban state courts in 2009, and 90
percent in federal court in 2014—are adjudicated through guil pleas.
4
Of
these, researchers estimate that more than 90 percent are a result of plea
bargaining—an informal and unregulated process by which prosecutors
and defense counsel negotiate charging and sentencing concessions in
exchange for guil pleas and waivers of constitutionally guaranteed trial
rights.
5
Indeed, by one estimate, a criminal case is disposed of by plea
bargaining every two seconds during a pical work day in America.
6
Negotiated deals to resolve criminal cases are so ubiquitous that Justice
By one estimate, a criminal case is
disposed of by plea bargaining every
two seconds during a typical work day
in America.
2 Vera Institute of Justice
Anthony Kennedy of the U.S. Supreme Court stated in 2012 that “criminal
justice today is for the most part a system of pleas, not a system of trials.
7
Plea deals—which are entirely within the discretion of a prosecutor to
offer (or accept)—pically include one or more of the following:
the dismissal of one or more charges, and/or agreement to a
conviction to a lesser offense (known as “charge bargaining”);
an agreement to a more lenient sentence, which can cover both pe
of sanction—custodial or communi-based—and length (known as
“sentence bargaining”); and
an agreement to stipulate to a version of events that omits certain
facts that would statutorily expose a person to harsher penalties
(known as “fact bargaining”).
8
Most criminal cases are resolved by plea bargaining.
Researchers
estimate that
more than
90%
of
criminal cases
that end in
conviction are
the result of plea
bargaining...
...a low-
visibility,
o-the-record,
and informal
process that
usually occurs
far from open
court.
In the Shadows: A Review of the Research on Plea Bargaining 3
Plea negotiations can be quick and straightforward or long and
complicated—or anywhere in between.
9
When there are no legal issues in
dispute, or in cases in which it is likely that the prosecution will prevail,
plea negotiations can be superficial—with people taking given deals
without any negotiation or counteroffer.
10
is is especially so in cases of
misdemeanors, infractions, and other lesser offenses for which there are
oen standard dispositions routinely offered and accepted with lile, if
any, actual deliberation. (See “Misdemeanor justice and plea bargains” on
page 16.) Oen, in more complex cases where available evidence is subject
to conflicting interpretations, and/or where sentencing stakes are higher,
negotiations can be lengthier—potentially occurring over many weeks and
months.
11
In a smaller proportion of cases, no actual offer or negotiation
occurs, and people plead guil without any specific promises or assurances
from the prosecution—called variously taking an “open plea,” taking a
“blind plea,” or “pleading to the sheet.
12
In whatever form it takes, plea bargaining remains a low-visibili, off-
the-record, and informal process that usually occurs in conference rooms
and courtroom hallways—or through private telephone calls or e-mails—
far away from the prying eyes and ears of open court.
13
Bargains are
usually struck with no witnesses present and made without investigation,
A system of plea bargaining.
P
l
e
a
B
a
r
g
a
i
n
i
n
g
P
l
e
a
B
a
r
g
a
i
n
i
n
g
P
l
e
a
B
a
r
g
a
i
n
i
n
g
P
l
e
a
B
a
r
g
a
i
n
i
n
g
P
l
e
a
B
a
r
g
a
i
n
i
n
g
P
l
e
a
B
a
r
g
a
i
n
i
n
g
P
l
e
a
B
a
r
g
a
i
n
i
n
g
P
l
e
a
B
a
r
g
a
i
n
i
n
g
4 Vera Institute of Justice
testimony, impartial fact-finding, or adherence to the required burden
of proof.
14
Moreover, lile to no documentation exists of the bargaining
process that takes place between initial charge and a persons formal
admission of guilt in open court, and final plea deals that close out cases
are themselves rarely wrien down or otherwise recorded.
15
As such, plea
deals, and the process that produces them, are largely unreviewable and
subject to lile public scrutiny.
16
us, despite the high frequency with
which plea deals are used, most people—aside from the usual courtroom
actors—understand neither the mechanics of plea bargaining nor the
reasons so many people decide to plead guil.
Plea bargaining has, however, become the central focus of a growing,
but still small, body of empirical research. In recent years, mounting
concerns about plea bargaining’s role in encouraging the widespread
forfeiture of constitutionally guaranteed trial rights and associated
procedural protections—and its critical role in eling mass incarceration—
has stimulated rther urgency in understanding how the process works.
Indeed, an array of questions regarding its fairness have emerged. Over the
In recent years, mounting concerns about
plea bargaining’s role in encouraging the
widespread forfeiture of constitutionally
guaranteed trial rights and associated
procedural protections—and its critical
role in fueling mass incarceration—
has stimulated further urgency in
understanding how the process works.
In the Shadows: A Review of the Research on Plea Bargaining 5
last few decades, prosecutorial leverage in plea negotiations has increased
exponentially as changes in substantive law have bolstered criminal
penalties and given prosecutors a wider range of choices to use when
filing charges (such as mandatory penalties, sentencing enhancements, and
more serious yet duplicative crimes already well covered by existing law).
But increased exposure to harsher penalties has not been matched with
increased procedural protections for defendants. Prosecutors’ wide powers
in plea bargaining still go largely unchecked, and there are no meaningl
oversight mechanisms or procedural safeguards to protect against unfair
or coercive practices, raising fears about arbitrariness and inequali. Given
this lack of regulation, concern has also grown over the extent to which
innocent people are regularly being induced to plead guil, as well as plea
bargaining’s role in perpetuating racial and ethnic disparities in criminal
case outcomes—for example, plea bargaining practices that send more
Black people to prison or jail than similarly situated white people.
17
Plea bargaining’s ll impact on the legal system and justice-involved
people remains unknown, but empirical research on this lile understood
yet immensely influential practice has begun to emerge. In order to provide
an accessible summary of existing research to policymakers and the public,
the Vera Institute of Justice (Vera) examined a body of empirical studies
that has developed around plea bargaining. Although this review is not
exhaustive, it provides a picture not only of the current state of scholarship
on plea bargaining, but also of the gaps in knowledge that must be filled.
As this report will discuss, studies appear to fall into seven main focus
areas, primarily examining either of two broad questions: (1) which factors
most influence the plea bargaining decision-making process?; and (2) what
is the impact of plea bargaining on case outcomes? e seven focus areas
covered by this report include the following.
Coercive factors. A number of studies examine whether the
punishing circumstances of pretrial detention or the threat of
onerous sentences—specifically, the death penal—play an outsized
role in inducing and expediting guil pleas.
Legal case characteristics. Another extensive body of work looks
at how legal case characteristics—severi of charge, prior record,
evidentiary factors—influence the pe of plea offers that are made.
6 Vera Institute of Justice
Systemic inequities. ere is a growing body of research
that considers the influence of demographic characteristics in
determining plea outcomes. ese studies primarily look at the
potential existence of conscious or unconscious biases that may
create disadvantage and inequali across race, ethnici, gender,
or age.
e criminal law. Another body of work looks at the organization,
structure, and content of the criminal code to understand how
the criminal law—including the existence and pe of structured
sentencing schemes—can affect the likelihood and substance of a
plea deal.
Caseloads. Some studies have tested the common assumption that
the frequent use of plea deals is, at least partly, a nction of heavy
caseloads among prosecutors and a resultant pressure to hasten the
disposal of as many cases as possible. (No research considered the
impact of defenders’ caseloads on plea bargaining.)
Trial penal. A substantial body of the literature explores the
so-called “trial penal” (or “plea discount”)—that is, the difference
between a criminal sentence produced by guil plea versus by trial.
Much of this work is narrowly based on the assumption that plea
decisions rely on anticipated trial outcomes, such that the higher
the penal (or larger the discount) the higher the likelihood of a
guil plea.
Innocence. Finally, given the potential benefits to people of
accepting a plea deal, a small body of research has considered the
extent to which innocent people may be coerced into pleading guil
to avoid receiving a “trial penal” if they fail to prove their cases
at trial.
Whether people are charged with serious crimes or low-level
misdemeanors, whether they are before busy ci courts or slower-paced
rural ones, they will likely resolve their cases by negotiated pleas. Plea
bargaining is the de facto system of justice in America. But despite decades
of scholarship, lile is ultimately known about how it works or why people
plead. is review of contemporary scholarship only offers mixed clues
about the criminal legal systems primary dispositional process. Although
In the Shadows: A Review of the Research on Plea Bargaining 7
there is evidence that most people receive more favorable sentencing
outcomes through plea bargaining than they would if they had taken their
cases to trial, the exact contours of how such bargains are reached, and
the factors—whether individual, legal, institutional, or demographic—
that ultimately play a key role in influencing plea outcomes remain both
ambiguous and opaque. What exists instead is a mix of complicated,
nuanced, and sometimes contradictory research findings.
In order to bring plea bargaining out of the shadows and ensure its
equitable use, more transparency is needed about the process by which
most cases are concluded so that safeguards can be put in place to protect
people from its misuse.
Whether people are charged with serious
crimes or low-level misdemeanors, whether
they are before busy city courts or slower-
paced rural ones, they will likely resolve
their cases by negotiated pleas.
8 Vera Institute of Justice
Law of plea bargaining: An overview
Plea bargaining emerged in the early years of the Republic
as a localized legal innovation that would eventually grow to
become—by the early 20th century—this country’s primary
method of criminal conviction.
a
But despite plea bargaining’s
long history at the center of American criminal justice, a set of
coherent rules governing its use has never emerged. Indeed,
for much of its history, plea bargaining remained a largely
unregulated and informal form of pretrial negotiation.
Although courts acknowledge plea bargaining as an “essential
component of the administration of justice”—or, more recently,
recognized that “it is the criminal justice system”—courts
have generally taken a hands-o approach in regulating its
use, leaving much discretion with prosecutors and defense
attorneys in shaping plea bargaining processes and outcomes.
b
This reticence is in part due to the approach that courts have
taken to analogize plea bargains to contracts in private law—
describing them as the same as “any other bargained-for
exchange” between autonomous actors who proceed from
a “mutuality of advantage”—even though prosecutor and
defendant have inherently unequal levels of power, particularly
when the accused is being held in jail pretrial.
c
Placing
substantive limits on bargaining tactics, according to this line
of thought, could potentially cast a chilling eect that might
foreclose plea negotiations altogether.
d
In only a few cases
have courts attempted to define plea bargaining’s contours or
set its outer bounds. Moreover, the few statutes and procedural
rules governing plea bargaining processes exert minimal
control over the direction and process of how plea bargains are
made and are largely silent regarding the actual substance
of plea bargains themselves.
e
However, in recent years, courts
have begun to fill the gaps in procedural rules with case law
clarifying the limits of prosecutorial discretion.
The constitutional law of plea bargaining
At the heart of plea bargaining law is the 1970 seminal case
of Brady v. United States. Long after plea bargaining had
become the norm in resolving the vast majority of criminal
cases, the Brady court formally recognized plea bargaining
as a constitutional method of criminal adjudication and set
out its minimum requirements.
f
To be constitutionally valid,
guilty pleas must be both “voluntary” and “knowing, intelligent
acts.”
g
However, courts have since defined both “voluntary” and
“knowing” in manners that are significantly dierent from what
the average person might suppose those terms to mean.
Voluntary. Although the voluntariness requirement supposedly
ensures that people are not misled, tricked, or otherwise
coerced into forgoing their constitutional right to trial by
pleading guilty, courts have still regularly avoided reckoning
with the inherent pressures of “the give-and-take of [the]
plea bargaining” process on people, including the inherently
coercive nature of pretrial detention.
h
Instead, they have
concluded that “the imposition of dicult choices [is] an
inevitable—and permissible—attribute of any legitimate system
which tolerates and encourages the negotiation of pleas.”
i
As a result, courts have decided that few actions will likely
amount to “improper pressure that would . . . overbear the
will of some innocent persons” such as to render a guilty plea
involuntary.
j
Prosecutors have been aorded wide latitude in
using every bargaining chip that is permissible by law when
trying to extract guilty pleas from people—including the threat
of indefinite pretrial detention—so long as the tactics do not
involve illegal fraud (such as threatening to use false testimony)
or outright physical harm, and so long as the bargain is
upheld.
k
These include promises of leniency if a plea is accepted
and threats of worse punishment, additional charges, or even
the prosecution and punishment of family members if a plea
deal is declined.
l
Knowing. Although courts have been clear that people must
possess several critical pieces of information in order to validly
plead guilty—the nature of the charges, the rights waived by
pleading guilty, and the sentence that will, or is likely to be,
imposed—the level of actual understanding that is required
is very minimal in practice.
m
Usually, a superficial judicial
inquiry—the “plea colloquy”—probes whether a person’s
plea bargaining choices are suciently informed; these are
typically highly scripted proceedings that are outlined in state
and federal procedural rules governing the formal entrance of
guilty pleas on the record in open court.
n
For the guilty plea
to pass constitutional muster, people typically need only to
provide short, often perfunctory, armative responses and a
basic explanation of the oense to which they are admitting.
o
Courts rarely conduct a deeper inquiry into whether people
fully comprehend the consequences of pleading guilty beyond
what is minimally required by the governing standard set out in
Brady and codified in the rules of criminal procedure.
p
There are also other larger structural curbs on what people can
know during the plea bargaining process. For example, people
do not have a right to know the full extent of the evidence, or
lack thereof, against them, because there is no constitutional
right to discovery in criminal cases.
q
The state is not required
to turn over evidence that may establish a person’s guilt and,
under United States v. Ruiz, it need not share material evidence
favorable to the defendant—a due process right established
under Brady v. Maryland and successor cases that only
attaches if a case goes to trial.
r
In the Shadows: A Review of the Research on Plea Bargaining 9
Recent developments in plea bargaining law
In recent years, the Supreme Court has become incrementally
more active in regulating the plea bargaining process—
particularly regarding the responsibilities of defense counsel.
This is in part due to an apparent change in approach.
Previously, the court viewed criminal trials as the normative
“touchstone” guiding its decisions around criminal law and
procedure—many of which centered on elaborating procedural
protections regarding the jury trial guarantee and attendant
rights.
s
But since 2010, in decidedly acknowledging plea
bargaining’s centrality to the criminal legal system, the court
has sought to extend legal protections to better ensure that
people receive competent legal advice and thus eective
bargaining by defense counsel during plea negotiations. For
example, in four cases decided from 2010 to 2017, the court
consistently held that people have a constitutional right to
eective legal counsel during the client counseling phase of the
plea bargaining process—a right that is normally associated
with criminal trials rather than a process that sits wholly apart
from trial proceedings.
t
In another area of development, courts have begun to
reconsider the scope of appeal rights that are waived when
people plead guilty. Generally, people forfeit a number of
rights simply by choosing to plead instead of going to trial. (For
example, people forfeit rights that would operate during the
trial, such as the right against self-incrimination or to confront
accusers, as well as certain claims that could have been raised
pretrial, such as the racial composition of the grand jury or
the credibility of prosecution witnesses.)
u
People can also
waive other rights through express waiver provisions that may
accompany a plea oer—although there may be some rights
that are not waivable.
v
The definitive scope of which rights are
impliedly waived by a guilty plea, as well as which rights one
can expressly waive, remains an open area of debate. But in
two recent cases, the Supreme Court held that:
a guilty plea alone—absent an express waiver—does not
bar people from challenging the constitutionality of their
convictions on direct appeal; and
people who suer ineective assistance of counsel retain
a right to appeal, even if their plea agreements specifically
waive that right.
w
In a separate development, echoing civil contract law, the U.S.
Second Circuit Court of Appeals nullified an appellate waiver in
2018 after finding that the plea agreement was not supported
by “consideration”—the benefit that each party gets or
expects as the part of an agreement. The defendant had been
sentenced to the statutory maximum and given no benefit of
sentence reductions that were available due to his acceptance
of responsibility and timely guilty plea, eectively giving him
nothing in exchange for waiving his right to appeal.
x
* Box notes at end of report.
10 Vera Institute of Justice
Factors that influence plea
bargaining
G
iven how central plea bargaining is to the nctioning of the
American criminal legal system, understanding how plea deals
are reached is deeply important. As explained in this section,
researchers have investigated the extent to which plea deals are shaped
by coercive factors (such as a persons pretrial detention status or the
possibili of a death penal sentence), the legal characteristics of a case
(such as the strength of the evidence, or the severi of the charges), and the
differential treatment of people based on their demographic characteristics
such as race, age, and gender.
Coercive factors
Of great concern to advocates, researchers, and defense counsel is the
inherently uneven playing field between accused and prosecutor in
plea bargaining situations—especially given the wide arsenal of tools,
particularly around charging, that prosecutors can use to increase their
Of great concern to advocates,
researchers, and defense counsel is
the inherently uneven playing field
between accused and prosecutor in plea
bargaining situations.
In the Shadows: A Review of the Research on Plea Bargaining 11
leverage in negotiations. Researchers have looked at how two coercive
factors in particular influence plea bargaining outcomes: pretrial detention
and the potential for a death sentence. ese studies have concluded that
these factors likely play a significant role in inducing guil pleas so that
people can obtain their liber—or even sustain their life.
Pretrial detention
e vast majori of people in local jails are detained pretrial, meaning that
they have not been convicted of any crime, are legally presumed innocent,
and are awaiting resolution of their criminal cases behind bars—most
oen because they cannot pay the bail set in their cases.
18
Pretrial detention
status has far-reaching consequences for justice-involved people. Concern
about this population—and the potential negative impacts of detention on
their criminal justice outcomes—has spawned a growing body of research
that has established a strong correlation between pretrial detention and an
increased likelihood of conviction, longer custodial sentences, and ture
system involvement.
19
While it has long been assumed that the pressure
and isolating circumstances of incarceration induce people to plead guil
more readily during the pretrial phase—potentially explaining why people
in pretrial detention are more likely to be convicted than those who are
released—researchers have aempted only recently to specifically examine
the influence of pretrial detention on a persons plea bargaining behavior.
20
Using a varie of different methods, this small body of scholarship has
established a strong association between pretrial detention and pleading
guil. For example, in a 2012 study examining 634 criminal cases in New
Jersey courts, researchers found that people who were detained pretrial
reached faster case dispositions, usually during the pre-indictment phase,
than people who were released, primarily because, as an interviewee
described it, “defendants plead guil to get out of jail . . . get time served
or to get it over with.
21
In a 2018 study looking at nearly 76,000 arrests in
Delaware, researchers similarly uncovered that pretrial detention increased
a persons likelihood of pleading guil by 46 percent—although there were
differences depending on the persons race.
22
Similar to previous research,
the study found that Black people were 10 percent less likely than white
people to enter into guil pleas.
23
To explain this finding, researchers
12 Vera Institute of Justice
have postulated that plea bargaining may be less common among Black
people because they may receive less favorable guil plea agreements
from prosecutors than do white people. Another reason may be that Black
people may be more distrustl of a legal system that disproportionately
and unfairly impacts them and, thus, less likely to strike a bargain.
24
Both the New Jersey and Delaware studies employed regression
analyses—a statistical method of measuring the relationship between
multiple variables—with only a limited set of controls based on data that
was available (for example, current charge aregated into broader offense
categories, criminal history, and demographic information). is means
that they did not account for a number of unobservable confounding
factors that may have also influenced peoples decisions to plead guil
(such as strength of evidence, quali of defense, individual cognitive
biases, or wealth). To correct for this potential bias and beer estimate
the causal effect of pretrial detention on a persons propensi to plead
guil, four recent studies conducted natural experiments—non-controlled
observational studies that exploit random assignment that occurs in
“nature” and which provide social scientists with a stronger inferential tool
to potentially improve the quali of their empirical inferences, particularly
when trying to infer causation.
25
ese studies exploited the random
or rotating assignment of bail judges and either variations in judicial
punitiveness in bail decisions or variations in peoples access to money bail
by day of the week.
26
Pretrial detention increases a person's likelihood of pleading
guilty by 46 percent.
plea
In the Shadows: A Review of the Research on Plea Bargaining 13
Consistent with other empirical research, all four studies confirmed
that a persons odds of conviction via guil plea increased when they
were held in pretrial detention—oen resulting in worse criminal justice
outcomes. For example, one study that looked at 331,971 criminal cases in
Philadelphia from 2006 to 2013 found that pretrial detention resulted in
a 4.7 percentage point increase in the likelihood of pleading guil among
people who probably would otherwise have been acquied, diverted,
or had their charges dropped.
27
Similarly, a second study that examined
nearly one million criminal cases over five years in New York Ci found
that detained people charged with felonies were 10 percent—and detained
people charged with misdemeanors were just over 7 percent—more likely
to plead guil compared to similarly-situated people who were released.
28
Again, not only did pretrial detention increase the odds of pleading guil,
but the study also found evidence that plea offers were less favorable for
people detained on felony charges in particular.
29
Plea deals for this group
were 10 percent less likely to include a charge reduction. e study also
found that pretrial detention may induce people to plead guil in cases
that would have been dismissed if those people had been released: 34
percent of released cases were dismissed, compared to only 19 percent of
cases among those detained.
30
Four studies confirmed that a person’s
odds of conviction via guilty plea
increased when they were held in pretrial
detention—often resulting in worse
criminal justice outcomes.
14 Vera Institute of Justice
e two other contemporaneous studies came to similar conclusions.
In a Harris Coun (Houston), Texas, study focusing only on misdemeanor
cases resolved from 2008 to 2013, researchers found that people who were
detained pretrial pleaded guil at a 25 percent higher rate than people
who were released and that data suested that 17 percent of the detained
people would likely not have been convicted at all had they been released
pretrial.
31
Conversely, the fourth study found that pretrial release reduced
a persons odds of pleading guil and improved plea outcomes. Aer
examining 420,000 felony and misdemeanor cases from Philadelphia and
Miami-Dade counties covering an eight-year study period, researchers
found that people who were released pretrial were nearly 11 percent
less likely to plead guil—although these estimates varied across race.
32
Released white people were nearly 20 percentage points less likely to plead
guil, while released Black people were only 12.2 percentage points less
likely to plead guil.
33
Of those who took a plea offer in the pretrial phase,
outcomes were still more favorable for people who had been released. ey
were more likely to be convicted of fewer offenses or convicted of a lesser
charge, suesting that release can improve a persons bargaining power in
plea negotiations.
34
In explaining their results, the researchers speculated that release
may enable people to engage in certain “prophylactic measures”—such as
preparing a more robust defense, engaging in treatment (for mental illness,
substance use disorder, or other behavioral health issues), or providing
restitution—that can help lead to charges being dismissed or encourage
more lenient treatment by prosecutors.
35
Some of these studies also found that the observed impacts of pretrial
detention—or release—on a persons plea bargaining propensi increased
in magnitude for two particular pes of people: those with no or limited
criminal justice histories and those charged with lower-level offenses.
For example, researchers in Harris Coun found that pretrial detention
more than doubled the likelihood of conviction for people charged with
first-time misdemeanors—the vast majori of whom had pleaded guil.
36
Similarly, the Philadelphia study found that pretrial detention among
people charged with misdemeanors led to statistically significant increases
in all observed negative outcomes, including conviction and guil pleas,
larger than those observed with felonies.
37
e Philadelphia/Miami study
also found that released people charged with misdemeanors were nearly
In the Shadows: A Review of the Research on Plea Bargaining 15
19 percent less likely to plead guil, while released people charged with
felonies were only 1.2 percent less likely to plead guil.
38
e study also
found that released people who did not have a recent prior offense were
more than 14 percent less likely to plead guil than similarly situated
people who were detained pretrial.
39
Put together, these findings are consistent with the theory that detained
people, who may have limited knowledge of the criminal legal system and/
or who are being detained for the first time, have strong incentives to cut
a quick deal in order to resolve their cases as soon as possible—especially
when it involves lower-stakes pey offenses. As was noted earlier, this
is because a plea deal is oen less “painl” in the short term because it
can get people out of jail if they are detained, purchase certain in what
can be a long and unpredictable criminal legal process, or sidestep the
possibili of harsher punishment that could result from trial. In line with
these observations about the long and painl process of justice system
involvement and detention—even for low-level offenses—the New York
Ci study found that people detained on misdemeanor charges pleaded
guil not only more frequently but also faster than people detained on
felony charges.
40
For incarcerated people charged with misdemeanors
whose cases continued beyond their first court appearance, the median
time between arraignment and case disposition was less than three weeks
at every predicted sentence length, but almost 50 days for people charged
with felonies in the shortest predicted sentence length category, growing
larger for groups with longer predicted sentence lengths.
41
Detained people have strong incentives to
cut a quick deal in order to resolve their
cases as soon as possible.
16 Vera Institute of Justice
Misdemeanor justice and plea bargains
More serious crimes—rape, murder, aggravated assault, etc.—
may be more likely to capture media and public attention, but
the American criminal legal system is, in fact, dominated by the
enforcement of a vast array of minor oenses located both in a
state’s criminal code and in local ordinances.
a
This enforcement
is both wide and deep, and it is the point of contact through
which most Americans experience the criminal legal system.
b
It touches everything from low-level crimes (such as disorderly
conduct, simple assault, petty theft, low-level fraud, illegal
trespass, prostitution, driving under the influence or on a
suspended license, and drug possession) to commonplace
behaviors deemed undesirable or “anti-social” that may either
be designated “civil” or “criminal” infractions (such as public
urination, public intoxication, unlicensed vending, littering,
grati, panhandling, jaywalking, and loitering).
c
Misdemeanors
can involve small harms, or no harms at all, and many are often
symptoms of larger social problems with which communities
grapple—including substance use disorders, mental illness,
domestic violence, and poverty.
d
Given the breadth of issues covered by minor oenses, it is
unsurprising that the national misdemeanor caseload volume
is large. According to a recent estimate, there were 13.2 million
misdemeanor cases filed in the United States in 2016—or 4,261
misdemeanor cases filed per 100,000 people; accounting for
76 percent of statewide criminal caseloads in 31 states.
e
It also
appears that the proportion of such cases among all criminal
prosecutions has remained relatively stable over the last
several decades.
f
Thus, the majority of plea bargains in America
are products of the country’s diverse and sprawling network
of lower criminal courts where minor charges are typically
adjudicated.
g
Despite the preponderance of minor charges in the criminal
legal system, a substantial proportion of empirical research
on plea bargaining either focuses on people charged (and
convicted) of felony oenses or comingles felonies and
misdemeanors, assuming that plea bargains operate with
similar goals—or under similar conditions and pressures—
across all types of oenses. But a small body of research—
mainly descriptive in nature—that has explored the real-life
operation of the lower courts suggests that guilty pleas
in misdemeanor and other petty cases are products of a
process that may be qualitatively dierent from that of most
serious oenses in ways that prevent aggregation in analysis,
particularly when trying to extend a generalizable explanation
of how plea bargains work.
h
This body of work confirms, for example, that in the world of
misdemeanors, infractions, and other lesser oenses, there are
often standard “deals”—baseline oers set to specific oense
types—that are routinely proposed and accepted that involve
little actual bargaining between parties.
i
For the accused,
these oers are hard-to-refuse dispositional deals that can
eectively end cases quickly, often as early as their first court
appearance.
j
These include:
case dismissal;
unconditional or conditional discharge;
a conviction of a civil (rather than criminal) infraction;
a sentence to time served (accounting for any time spent in
custody leading up to the court appearance); or
a community-based sentence of minimal length.
k
Thus, contrary to the notion that prosecutors and defendants/
defense counsel “bargain toward settlement in the shadow
of expected trial outcomes,” in these cases, initial charging
decisions—which are in the prosecutor’s discretion—determine
the type of plea deal that settles a particular matter under
established court practices, prosecutor oce policies, or
defense bar customs around similar cases in a particular
jurisdiction.
l
Researchers have proposed two main explanations of why
cases related to petty oenses are resolved quickly:
Process costs. Prosecutors, defense counsel, defendants,
and judges all put a premium on speed because of the
enormous scale of misdemeanor dockets across the country
and the high process costs that misdemeanor trials present
to all parties when compared to the low-stakes nature of the
oenses.
m
With a criminal legal process that is both slow and
unpredictable, costs for the accused include uncertainty of
outcome and a protracted pretrial and trial process that can
span many days, weeks, or even months—one that may be
experienced in part, or wholly, behind bars and away from
their families, jobs, and community responsibilities.
n
Thus,
paying the price of a guilty plea may be, counterintuitively,
the most “rational” option whether a person is guilty or not.
o
Managerial justice. Another body of scholarly work
suggests that prosecutors are not actually trying to
maximize punishment or even secure a conviction when
oering standard plea deals to quickly resolve misdemeanor
and other petty cases.
p
Instead, prosecutors use various
adjudicatory tools that avoid formal punishment but enable
them to document a person’s criminal justice encounters
and track behavior over time (such as conditional discharge
or an adjournment in contemplation of a dismissal) so that
law enforcement agencies (such as police, prosecutors, and
courts) have a record to use in calibrating future responses.
q
In the Shadows: A Review of the Research on Plea Bargaining 17
The impact of death penalty sentences
Fears about the coercive nature of plea bargaining are perhaps most
pronounced in cases in which prosecutors have the option to pursue a
death penal conviction. Critics note that when taking cases to trial that
may result in a sentence to death, people may be more likely to accept a
plea deal that they would otherwise have rejected—due either to the harsh
terms of the sentence and/or their factual innocence.
42
e threat of the
death penal is used as leverage by prosecutors in bargaining, and both
prosecutors and defense lawyers agree that the specter of a death penal
puts prosecutors in a uniquely strong position.
43
Although analysis of
actual cases is hampered by a relatively small sample to draw from, the
quantitative studies suest that the option to pursue a death penal has a
significant effect on plea bargaining.
One such study conducted a natural experiment capitalizing on the
1995 reinstatement of the death penal in New York State to analyze
changes in case outcomes before and aer the law change.
44
e study
found that people charged in murder cases were 25 percent more likely
to plead guil to their charges following the law change, regardless
of whether the prosecutor had explicitly filed a notice to pursue the
death penal in the case.
45
e reintroduction of the death penal, the
researchers concluded, made people less likely to be offered a charge
reduction (pically the more advantageous pe of plea offer) and more
likely to take a sentence deal—suesting that prosecutors did indeed
have—and use—greater power in plea bargaining.
46
A study from Georgia
Prosecutors and defense lawyers agree
that the specter of a death penalty puts
prosecutors in a uniquely strong position.
18 Vera Institute of Justice
produced similar results.
47
e study looked at eight years of murder
cases from across the state that met the criteria necessary to be tried as
capital cases. e researcher compared the outcomes of cases in which
prosecutors pursued the death penal with those in which they did not,
using sophisticated statistical techniques to control for a varie of case
characteristics. ey found that, all else being equal, people charged in
murder cases were approximately 20 to 25 percentage points more likely
to plead guil when faced with the death penal.
48
Put another way, the
study suests that when prosecutors actively pursue a death penal,
people in an additional two out of every 10 cases are deterred from going
to trial.
49
ese findings have been supported nationally. A comparison of 33
counties—some with the death penal, some without—found that, when
the death penal was available, 19 percent of first degree murder cases
were resolved with a guil plea leading to a prison sentence of more than
20 years; in counties without the death penal, this was true for only 5
percent of cases.
50
is large difference in outcomes remained statistically
significant even when controlling for other case characteristics. In pleading
guil to a life sentence, people are relinquishing their right to appeal, the
chance of an acquial, and the possibili of a shorter sentence; avoiding
the death penal is used as a “substantial incentive” to encourage people to
make this otherwise unappealing decision.
51
People are 25 percentage points more likely to plead guilty when
faced with the death penalty.
death
sentence
plea
In the Shadows: A Review of the Research on Plea Bargaining 19
Legal characteristics
Researchers have looked at how the application of plea bargaining, and the
potential associated discount in punishment, might vary in relation to the
legal characteristics of a case. Of these, the strength of the evidence, the
severi of the charges, and the accuseds criminal history have garnered
most aention.
Strength of evidence
e strength of the evidence against a person charged with a crime is
perhaps the most salient factor prosecutors say they consider when
deciding whether a plea deal should be extended. Interviews and
surveys conducted with prosecutors have aempted to shed light on the
relationship between the strength of the evidence in a case and the plea
bargaining process, oen testing the general assumption that prosecutors
will offer more lenient deals when the evidence is weaker or harsher deals
when the evidence is stronger. In a survey of 166 prosecutors in three
southern states, 82 percent of respondents indicated that they would
reduce the harshness of the plea if the evidence was weak, and 38 percent
indicated that they would increase the harshness of the plea if the evidence
was strong.
52
However, a different survey study asked nearly 400 prosecutors to
respond to hypothetical legal cases and found that the presence or absence
of evidence had very lile effect on prosecutors’ likelihood to recommend
a plea deal, with plea offers being extended in nearly all cases.
53
Again, the
presence of evidence decreased the leniency of offers recommended by
prosecutors, but only by a small amount.
54
e authors noted, however,
that findings from hypothetical cases such as these—with the absence of
real-life court and caseload pressures—may not easily generalize to actual
practice.
55
As such, a number of studies have aempted to examine the
relationship between plea bargaining and evidence strength in real cases.
An analysis of cases relating to person and proper offenses
(misdemeanor and felony) filed in one jurisdiction across multiple years
sought to determine the relationship between evidence strength and
plea bargaining using quantitative analysis.
56
As a proxy for evidence
20 Vera Institute of Justice
strength, the research considered the total number of pieces of evidence
listed in a case and the number of pieces of evidence per intake charge.
57
While controlling for a number of other case and defendant variables, the
researchers found a significant relationship between strength of evidence
and plea offers—but not on all metrics. e strength of the evidence was
unrelated to both the seriousness of the top plea offer charge made by
prosecutors and the length of sentence aached to it. Stronger evidence
was, however, associated with increases in the number of charges included
in the plea offer, the total sentence possible if convicted for all charges,
and the likelihood that the prosecutor would recommend incarceration.
58
A 2015 study of felony drug cases in New York Ci similarly assessed
the relationship between evidence and plea bargain outcomes while
controlling for other legal and extralegal factors.
59
e researchers found
that when factors indicating strong evidence were present, the accused
were less likely to receive reduced plea charge offers and more likely to
receive custodial, rather than noncustodial, sentence recommendations.
60
However, while significant, the size of these relationships was smaller than
the researchers had expected.
61
Confession evidence is considered extremely persuasive at trial, which
could make it an important factor during plea bargaining.
62
An analysis
of more than 500 felony cases from two New York counties showed that
people who confessed (either lly or partially) overwhelmingly pled guil
(more than 97 percent), but received lower plea discounts than others
(people not interviewed by police and people who denied guilt).
63
As a
rther analysis, researchers in this study coded the apparent strength
of the evidence in each case. Contrary to expectations—and to findings
from earlier studies—cases with stronger evidence received greater plea
discounts than cases with weaker evidence.
64
e researchers proposed
that, because in the coun they studied the prosecutor’s office had a
known policy stating that no offer would be beer than the initial offer,
people with strong cases against them who knew about the policy were
more likely to accept the first plea deal offered.
65
Researching the relationship between plea bargaining and the strength
of the evidence in real cases presents specific and significant challenges:
the strength of evidence is unlikely to be recorded in ways that are
amenable to large scale analysis and is difficult to quanti in a meaningl
way.
66
ere are nuances to evidence that are difficult to assess and
In the Shadows: A Review of the Research on Plea Bargaining 21
combine in the aregate. For example, eyewitness testimony may vary
in its persuasiveness if it comes from an undercover police officer or a
witness to the incident, and witnesses may be afforded different levels of
credibili based on their demographics and relationship to the accused.
67
Furthermore, the strength of a specific piece of evidence is not necessarily
static and may decay over time as witnesses fall away or forget important
details or an investigation throws doubt on what first appeared to be
important evidence.
68
Charge severity
e seriousness of the current offense is a key consideration when
contemplating plea agreements. Research suests that the likelihood of
a person pleading guil and the leniency of the plea deal offered to them
by prosecutors may both be influenced by the severi of the charges
the person faces.
69
A study of 200 cases of drunk driving in a southern
California coun in 1993, for example, analyzed the relationship between
multiple legal and extralegal case characteristics and case outcomes and
found that charge severi was the largest predictor of pleas; people with
more severe charges were less likely to plead guil.
70
e researchers
suested that when the stakes of conviction are higher, there is greater
incentive to take ones chances in court.
71
Another study of 464 felony
The relationship between charge severity and plea likelihood.
increase in
charge severity
decreased likelihood
of pleading guilty
!
22 Vera Institute of Justice
cases from Virginia in the late 1970s, which also controlled for many
demographic and case characteristics, similarly found that when charged
with offenses that could result in longer sentences, people became less
likely to plead guil.
72
Studies of whether a deal is reached, however, say nothing of the
leniency (or lack thereof) in the plea offers made by prosecutors. A large-
scale study of federal cases that resulted in conviction in 2001 (excluding
cases resolved through trial) found that more serious charges were
associated with a greater likelihood of charge reductions.
73
e researchers
theorized that this may be, at least in part, because more serious crimes—
and those with more filing charges—allow more opportunities for
reduction than do lower-level crimes.
74
Criminal history
Peoples conviction histories are oen used to guide sentencing decisions
and are sometimes explicitly included in prosecutors’ plea deal guidelines.
75
As such, it would make sense that criminal histories might exert a
significant influence on the plea bargaining process. Although researchers
frequently control for criminal history when examining the relationship of
other legal and extralegal factors with plea bargaining outcomes, it is less
frequently the subject of direct investigation itself.
Research conducted in coun criminal courts in Pennsylvania (using
data from 1997 through 2000) and, in a separate study, federal criminal
courts (using data from 2000 through 2002) found that people with more
substantial criminal histories are penalized less for going to trial than those
without.
76
at is to say, the difference between sentences resulting from
a jury trial compared to those imposed through plea bargaining becomes
smaller as peoples criminal histories increase. Researchers offer several
plausible explanations for this relationship. First, a jury trial may provide
an opportuni for defense counsel to argue that a persons criminal history
is less serious or meaningl than it may seem on paper and to present
evidence of the accuseds good character.
77
Second, it is possible that, for
those with longer criminal histories, prosecutors offer less lenient plea
deals or encourage people to plead to the charges set at arraignment.
e sentence they receive as a result is unlikely to differ markedly from
In the Shadows: A Review of the Research on Plea Bargaining 23
a sentence they might receive at trial. Although the first explanation is
difficult to quanti and study, research has lent some support to the
second—although, as elsewhere, the findings have been mixed.
A large study of people who were sentenced in federal cases in 2001
found that criminal history had no influence on the reduction of charges
during plea bargaining.
78
However, other studies—which similarly
analyze the relationship between criminal history and plea bargaining
(while controlling for other important variables)—have found that more
substantial criminal histories are associated with harsher plea deals,
at least for some charge pes. For example, a study of nearly 160,000
misdemeanor cases prosecuted by the District Aorney of New York
Coun found that, holding other case and defendant characteristics
constant, people with longer criminal histories were more likely than
people with shorter histories to receive a plea offer that included
incarceration.
79
Similarly, statistical analysis conducted in one unnamed
jurisdiction found that—for all charge pes studied (proper, personal,
and drug)—more serious criminal histories were associated with an
When evaluating the association between
people’s criminal histories and the
likelihood of receiving a plea deal—or the
harshness or leniency of that deal—it is
important to keep in mind that criminal
histories themselves are subject to deep
rooted racial biases within the criminal
legal system.
24 Vera Institute of Justice
increased likelihood that the plea deal would include incarceration and
with smaller reductions in the total possible sentence a person might
serve.
80
However, more serious criminal histories were only found to be
associated with smaller reductions in the seriousness of the top charges
and smaller reductions in the total number of charges for drug offenses.
81
Criminal histories are, of course, not a neutral factor, but rather one
rooted in the nations historic and continuing systemic racism. When
evaluating the association between peoples criminal histories and the
likelihood of receiving a plea deal—or the harshness or leniency of that
deal—it is important to keep in mind that criminal histories themselves
are not created with objective neutrali and are subject to deep rooted
racial biases within the criminal legal system.
82
Indeed, research conducted
in New York Ci found that people with longer criminal histories were
more likely to have their cases dismissed, the result of a dynamic in
which police will more readily arrest people—especially Black and Latinx
people—whether or not they have sufficient evidence for a prosecutor to
file charges.
83
Systemic inequities
As described below, the complete lack of transparency and scrutiny
inherent in Americas reliance on plea bargaining—combined with the
U.S. justice systems long history of prejudicial operations—leaves the
administration of plea deals open to bias. Relatedly, people may have
widely diverging levels of trust in the system, which influence their
willingness to accept a plea deal or face trial. As such, researchers have
investigated the degree to which plea offers are affected by demographic
factors, notably the race, gender, and age of the accused.
Racism and race inequity
Consistent with the experiences of Black and Latinx people throughout
the criminal legal system, several studies have found that people of color
are oen treated less favorably than white people during the plea bargain
process. For example, two studies of data from the New York Coun
District Aorney’s office found that Black people were significantly less
Challenges in researching plea bargaining
Guilty pleas constitute the main mechanism for case resolution
in American criminal courts. Researching the role and
administration of plea bargaining in this process has, however,
proved remarkably dicult.
The process of plea bargaining is rarely recorded in any formal
documentation and, when it is, these records are often not
made available to researchers.
a
Simply asking prosecutors
about the plea bargaining process undoubtedly has some
value, but researchers have found that there can be substantial
dierences between lawyers’ perceptions of their work, their
values, and their actual behavior in plea negotiations.
b
More
importantly, asking prosecutors about plea bargaining ignores
the voice of the person most aected by the bargain, and few
studies focus on the perspective of the person pleading guilty.
c
With scant records and limited trust in the validity of self-
reported behavior, researchers have attempted to infer
details of the plea bargaining process by looking at case
outcomes. Although there are many variations in methodology,
researchers commonly seek evidence of charge bargaining
by comparing the initial charges a person faced with those
to which they ultimately pled guilty. Evidence of sentence
bargaining is often inferred by comparing the punishments
given following a plea with those given following a trial
for the same charges.
d
There are limitations to both approaches.
Inferring that charge bargaining has occurred on the basis
of a reduction in charges (either number or seriousness) is
problematic because charges may be reduced prior to case
disposition for a number of reasons not necessarily related
to plea bargaining.
e
For example, charges could be reduced
to compensate for mistakes or biases that led to initial
overcharging or in light of new evidence collected during an
investigation and not because of any plea deal.
f
Quantifying
the presence and impact of sentence bargaining is also
problematic. Cases that go to trial dier in significant, hard-to-
measure ways from cases that result in a plea, and researchers
are inconsistent in how they actually measure sentences—
disagreeing on whether to include noncustodial punishments
as “zero-length” sentences, sometimes counting acquittals in a
similar way, and diering in whether to account for time served
in pretrial detention.
g
As prosecutors may employ dierent bargaining strategies in
dierent circumstances, it is preferable that research studies
attempt to measure both charge bargaining and sentence
bargaining, but this does not always happen.
h
Furthermore,
researchers have also noted that, at least in the federal
court system, there is an additional common form of plea
bargaining in which the prosecutor and defense may reach a
deal over the key “sentencing facts” of a case—agreeing to
omit certain facts about a case that would statutorily expose
a person to harsher sentences. This process is perhaps even
harder to record, identify, or measure than charge or sentence
bargaining, and it has received even less research attention as
a result.
i
Perhaps the most confounding aspect of plea bargaining, next
to the lack of available records, is its granularity: the type of
plea bargain favored (charge, sentence, or fact), the frequency
with which it occurs, and the degree of impact it has on
sentencing outcomes can all vary by the specific jurisdiction,
prosecutor, and charges associated with a case.
j
Studies
that aggregate data across charge types, courts, or type of
plea bargaining may, therefore, dilute any evidence of plea
bargaining and its impact, hiding any important associations;
conversely, studies that focus on specific charges, courts, or
metrics are likely to be limited in their generalizability.
k
In the Shadows: A Review of the Research on Plea Bargaining 25
increased likelihood that the plea deal would include incarceration and
with smaller reductions in the total possible sentence a person might
serve.
80
However, more serious criminal histories were only found to be
associated with smaller reductions in the seriousness of the top charges
and smaller reductions in the total number of charges for drug offenses.
81
Criminal histories are, of course, not a neutral factor, but rather one
rooted in the nations historic and continuing systemic racism. When
evaluating the association between peoples criminal histories and the
likelihood of receiving a plea deal—or the harshness or leniency of that
deal—it is important to keep in mind that criminal histories themselves
are not created with objective neutrali and are subject to deep rooted
racial biases within the criminal legal system.
82
Indeed, research conducted
in New York Ci found that people with longer criminal histories were
more likely to have their cases dismissed, the result of a dynamic in
which police will more readily arrest people—especially Black and Latinx
people—whether or not they have sufficient evidence for a prosecutor to
file charges.
83
Systemic inequities
As described below, the complete lack of transparency and scrutiny
inherent in Americas reliance on plea bargaining—combined with the
U.S. justice systems long history of prejudicial operations—leaves the
administration of plea deals open to bias. Relatedly, people may have
widely diverging levels of trust in the system, which influence their
willingness to accept a plea deal or face trial. As such, researchers have
investigated the degree to which plea offers are affected by demographic
factors, notably the race, gender, and age of the accused.
Racism and race inequity
Consistent with the experiences of Black and Latinx people throughout
the criminal legal system, several studies have found that people of color
are oen treated less favorably than white people during the plea bargain
process. For example, two studies of data from the New York Coun
District Aorney’s office found that Black people were significantly less
Challenges in researching plea bargaining
Guilty pleas constitute the main mechanism for case resolution
in American criminal courts. Researching the role and
administration of plea bargaining in this process has, however,
proved remarkably dicult.
The process of plea bargaining is rarely recorded in any formal
documentation and, when it is, these records are often not
made available to researchers.
a
Simply asking prosecutors
about the plea bargaining process undoubtedly has some
value, but researchers have found that there can be substantial
dierences between lawyers’ perceptions of their work, their
values, and their actual behavior in plea negotiations.
b
More
importantly, asking prosecutors about plea bargaining ignores
the voice of the person most aected by the bargain, and few
studies focus on the perspective of the person pleading guilty.
c
With scant records and limited trust in the validity of self-
reported behavior, researchers have attempted to infer
details of the plea bargaining process by looking at case
outcomes. Although there are many variations in methodology,
researchers commonly seek evidence of charge bargaining
by comparing the initial charges a person faced with those
to which they ultimately pled guilty. Evidence of sentence
bargaining is often inferred by comparing the punishments
given following a plea with those given following a trial
for the same charges.
d
There are limitations to both approaches.
Inferring that charge bargaining has occurred on the basis
of a reduction in charges (either number or seriousness) is
problematic because charges may be reduced prior to case
disposition for a number of reasons not necessarily related
to plea bargaining.
e
For example, charges could be reduced
to compensate for mistakes or biases that led to initial
overcharging or in light of new evidence collected during an
investigation and not because of any plea deal.
f
Quantifying
the presence and impact of sentence bargaining is also
problematic. Cases that go to trial dier in significant, hard-to-
measure ways from cases that result in a plea, and researchers
are inconsistent in how they actually measure sentences—
disagreeing on whether to include noncustodial punishments
as “zero-length” sentences, sometimes counting acquittals in a
similar way, and diering in whether to account for time served
in pretrial detention.
g
As prosecutors may employ dierent bargaining strategies in
dierent circumstances, it is preferable that research studies
attempt to measure both charge bargaining and sentence
bargaining, but this does not always happen.
h
Furthermore,
researchers have also noted that, at least in the federal
court system, there is an additional common form of plea
bargaining in which the prosecutor and defense may reach a
deal over the key “sentencing facts” of a case—agreeing to
omit certain facts about a case that would statutorily expose
a person to harsher sentences. This process is perhaps even
harder to record, identify, or measure than charge or sentence
bargaining, and it has received even less research attention as
a result.
i
Perhaps the most confounding aspect of plea bargaining, next
to the lack of available records, is its granularity: the type of
plea bargain favored (charge, sentence, or fact), the frequency
with which it occurs, and the degree of impact it has on
sentencing outcomes can all vary by the specific jurisdiction,
prosecutor, and charges associated with a case.
j
Studies
that aggregate data across charge types, courts, or type of
plea bargaining may, therefore, dilute any evidence of plea
bargaining and its impact, hiding any important associations;
conversely, studies that focus on specific charges, courts, or
metrics are likely to be limited in their generalizability.
k
Rarely
recorded.
Limitations to
researching charge,
sentence, and fact
bargaining.
Practical and
policy variations by
jurisdiction, prosecutor,
and charges.
A number of issues complicate research into plea bargaining.
26 Vera Institute of Justice
likely than white people to receive reduced charges and more likely to
receive custodial sentence offers, aer controlling for various demographic
and case factors.
84
More punitive plea deals for Black and Latinx people, as compared to
white people, may partly be explained as the result of worse treatment
during other stages of case processing. Most notably, they are more likely
to be held in jail pretrial than similarly situated white people, which can
lead to a range of worse case outcomes.
85
People held in pretrial detention
are more likely to plead guil—in part to hasten their release—and to face
harsher sentencing decisions. In this way, people of color, and especially
Black and Latinx people, face “cumulative disadvantage” in the criminal
legal system, and their increased vulnerabili to pretrial detention in turn
increases the likelihood that they will receive a worse plea deal than white
people (while controlling for other factors such as charge severi and
criminal histories).
86
Several studies suest that harsher plea deals for Black people are
not simply a byproduct of pretrial detention, however. Researchers have
found evidence to support the hypothesis that prosecutors’ biases may
lead them to use a persons race as a proxy for criminali or risk.
87
A study
of misdemeanor and felony cases in Wisconsin from 1999 to 2006 found
that white people were 25 percent more likely than Black people to have
Consistent with the experiences of Black
and Latinx people throughout the criminal
legal system, several studies have found
that people of color are treated less
favorably than white people during the
plea bargain process.
In the Shadows: A Review of the Research on Plea Bargaining 27
their initial charges reduced, but only in cases where the accused did not
have a criminal history; when people had a prior record, this dispari
disappeared.
88
Similarly, white people were found to receive preferential
plea bargaining treatment over Black people in cases of low-level offenses,
but not for more serious charges.
89
e researchers propose that when
people are accused of serious crimes or have criminal histories, they have
already proven the risk they pose and are treated equally harshly.
90
Absent
this information, prosecutors may find their perceptions influenced by a
persons race. Other studies have found a similar difference in treatment
favoring white people over people of color, and especially Black and Latinx
people, to be most pronounced for misdemeanor and lower-level felony
charges.
91
In misdemeanor cases in New York Ci, for example, the odds
of receiving a custodial plea offer were almost 70 percent greater for Black
people than white people.
92
It is difficult to tell how and to what extent race influences plea offers
because of inconsistencies in study design. One study of felony cases
from Cook Coun (Chicago) in the early 1990s did not find evidence
of racial bias in plea bargaining; however, the study only measured
reductions in the number of charges people faced when pleading guil—a
narrow definition of plea bargaining.
93
Other studies have only found
an association between race and plea bargaining for specific charge
categories.
94
For example, Black and Latinx people have been found to be
significantly less likely than white people to receive charge reductions for
The odds of receiving a plea oer that includes incarceration are
almost 70 percent greater for Black people than white people.
plea
28 Vera Institute of Justice
weapons offenses.
95
Research that aregates different pes of cases—
combining misdemeanor and felony cases, or multiple offense pes—in
analysis may miss these important relationships, masking significant
associations between race and the plea deals people are offered.
96
Other variables that may not be easily recorded or analyzed—such
as the race of the alleged victim—may mediate the relationship between
race and the plea offers people receive. For example, research conducted
in Rhode Island reinforces the importance of social context, which many
studies are not able to meaninglly consider.
97
is study of plea offers
in drunk driving cases found that the harshness with which people of
color were treated relative to white people was most pronounced when
the alleged crime was commied in “advantaged” (wealthy and racially
homogeneous white) areas, such as suburbs.
98
Adding rther complexi, a recent study of nearly 180,000
misdemeanor and felony cases in New York Ci found that, although
Black people were only slightly less likely than white people to receive
reductions in their charges, the timing of these reductions were notably
different, with important implications.
99
For white people, charge
reductions happened most oen at disposition—likely reflecting leniency
on the part of court actors and the negotiation of a plea deal; for Black
people, charge reductions happened most frequently at initial case
screening, suesting that this was corrective action to perceived bias in
overcharging by the police and not part of a plea deal.
100
is provides an
important reminder that, having already faced discrimination by the police,
people of color oen enter the court system at a disadvantage—one for
Research into biases in plea bargaining
will be hampered if it does not take into
consideration biases in initial charging.
In the Shadows: A Review of the Research on Plea Bargaining 29
which equitable treatment during plea bargaining, were it to exist, would
not correct.
101
In short, research into biases in plea bargaining will be
hampered if it does not take into consideration biases in initial charging.
102
Disparate treatment by gender
Research has also examined possible gender-based outcomes in plea
negotiations. Aempts to study the relationship between gender and
the use of plea bargains have faced methodological challenges in finding
sufficiently large and directly comparable samples of men and women, as
women are arrested less frequently than men and pically for different,
less violent, charges.
103
When researchers have managed to overcome these
specific challenges, the results have been mixed. Several studies conducted
using data from the 1970s through the 1990s failed to find any significant
relationship between gender and the use of plea bargaining.
104
More recently, researchers have published several studies that suest
some association may exist, with women receiving more lenient treatment
than men.
105
A study examining felony and misdemeanor data from 2000
Race and gender both influence plea oers.
Harshest plea treatment
for Black men
Most lenient plea treatment
for white women
Harsher for men
More lenient for women
!
30 Vera Institute of Justice
through 2006 in Wisconsin, for example, found that women were more
likely than men to have their top charges reduced or dismissed and that
this dispari was greatest for people with no prior convictions and in
cases where the top charge was a misdemeanor or low-level felony.
106
e
researchers hypothesize that, in these cases, with lile information to guide
them, prosecutors may be falling back on gendered assumptions about
“risk” and “criminali” and treating men more harshly as a result.
107
e
researchers also looked to see how gender interacted with race and found
that white women were afforded the most lenient treatment through plea
bargaining while Black men received the harshest.
108
A study of federal
court sentencing data for cases terminating in 2001 (nearly 46,000 cases)
similarly found that, overall, men were more than 30 percent less likely than
women to receive a charge reduction, although the study did not review the
data for evidence of a race/gender interaction and instead focused on which
individual factor looked more significant across all transactions.
109
Several explanations have been posited to account for relatively
preferential treatment of women: prosecutors may hold gendered
stereopes that view women as less culpable and as less of a risk to
socie, or women may invoke “selective sympathy” among court actors.
110
Researchers have noted, however, that many of the reasons invoked for
more lenient treatment of women—such as consideration of their mental
health or parental status—could equally be applied to men and, instead of
questioning why women are seemingly treated more leniently, the more
pertinent question should be “why are men treated so harshly. . . ?”
111
Dierential plea bargaining experiences by age
Research into the relationship between a persons age and the use of
plea bargains has oen focused on the different ways in which young
people may approach and respond to plea deals compared to their older
counterparts; qualitative and experimental studies find that younger
people may be more likely than older people to accept a plea offer and
give less consideration to the potential negative consequences of the
decision.
112
Gaps in young peoples understanding of a plea offer may not
be adequately addressed by their aorneys, and they may not receive
important information about alternatives to pleading guil.
113
Interviews
In the Shadows: A Review of the Research on Plea Bargaining 31
conducted with 18 aorneys in one jurisdiction asked respondents about
their most recent juvenile client.
114
Nearly all aorneys believed that their
clients understood that they were giving up a trial, but fewer than half
reported that they had explicitly discussed this with their clients. Many
of the aorneys waited to discuss waivers of rights until aer their client
had stated that they would accept a plea, and only about a quarter of the
aorneys surveyed discussed collateral consequences—a vast array of
post-sentence civil penalties, disqualifications, or disabilities that flow
from criminal convictions—with their clients.
115
Interviews conducted
with adolescent and adult participants in an alternative to incarceration
program in New York Ci found that, based on self-reports, adolescents
had less time to make their plea decisions than adults (frequently as lile
as an hour) and that juveniles met with their aorneys fewer times than
adults did.
116
Although there are reasons to believe that young people differ in
their understanding of and responses to plea offers, the small number
of aempts made to quantitatively measure differences in plea bargain
outcomes between older and younger people have yielded mixed results—
potentially due to methodological issues. (See “Challenges in researching
plea bargaining” on page 25.) A study of misdemeanor and felony cases
from one jurisdiction across multiple years found mixed results and
illustrates the complexi of the issue: the relationship between age and
the favorabili of the plea deal reached varied by charge pe (person,
proper, or drug) and by the measure of plea deal (changes in the statutory
rank of the top charge, changes in the possible sentence length of the top
charge, changes in the possible sentence length of all charges combined,
and prosecutor sentence recommendations).
117
For some charge pes in
some measures, increased defendant age was associated with greater plea
discounts and, for other pes, lesser.
118
Possible relationships between
age and plea bargaining may, therefore, be too nuanced and fine-grained
to identi using aregated datasets. And unlike the studies of juveniles,
this study did not focus on the people affected by the bargain or their
motivations for making it, but instead on prosecutorial decisions.
32 Vera Institute of Justice
The criminal law
In addition to the significant influence of legal case characteristics;
systemic biases associated with race, age, and gender; and pretrial detention
status on plea bargaining practices, the law itself also plays a critical role in
what agreement can be reached. Although prosecutors’ charging authori
arguably concentrates a significant amount of adjudicative power into
their hands—through their abili to control and manipulate a persons
sentencing exposure by way of the charges they pursue and then revise as a
result of plea bargaining—this power is neither subject to any consistently
identifiable standards or rules, nor aer-the-fact review.
119
(See “Law of plea
bargaining: An overview” on page 8.) As a result, plea bargaining is oen
regarded as an unwrien, latent body of law or, at its worst, a practice
that inhabits a “lawless” space, where the risk of capricious and arbitrary
prosecutorial decision making—including invidious discrimination
remains ever present.
120
is may be, in part, why critics of plea bargaining
oen refer to it as a practice operating “beyond the shadow of the law”—
both driven and governed by the vast, opaque, and unregulated exercise of
prosecutorial discretion.
121
Given this, the studies discussed below demonstrate that prosecutorial
decisions in the plea bargaining context do not operate in a vacuum.
Rather, prosecutors’ plea bargaining decisions remain contingent on both
the architecture and substance of the criminal code. e law provides the
essential framework for plea negotiations. ese studies, therefore, stand
for the proposition that established legal rules still maer in defining the
contours of plea bargaining behavior.
The criminal code
In two companion descriptive studies that looked at charge bargaining
practices in North Carolina in 1999 and 2000—one conducted in 2006
and another in 2007—researchers confirmed that the substantive criminal
law still maers in determining plea bargaining outcomes.
122
In the first
study, the researchers not only found that charge reduction was a frequent
occurrence, but they also discovered that both charge reduction rate and
magnitude increased where the criminal code offered prosecutors and
In the Shadows: A Review of the Research on Plea Bargaining 33
defense lawyers a deeper set of plausible charges as landing spots in plea
negotiations.
123
At the same time, the researchers also uncovered that
parties were less likely to agree on a particular charge reduction—thus
minimizing the rate of overall charge movement in plea negotiations—
when there were greater differences between the potential sentences that
aached to available charging options.
124
While these two main findings held true across both serious and
nonserious charges, charge reductions were found to be more common
among serious cases than in less serious ones—with the highest rates
observed among the highest felony categories for which prison sentences
were presumptively aached.
125
However, the frequency with which cases
moved from the original charge to a lesser one—and the distance in charge
and sentence severi between them—varied by the specific charge pes
examined by the study (assault, robbery, burglary, kidnapping, and cocaine-
related).
126
For example, the kidnapping group had fewer charge bargains
because the criminal code contained few charging options, while the large
sentencing gap between potential sentences across the felony-misdemeanor
line for the burglary group inhibited the number of plea deals despite the
wide availabili of many charging options.
127
e researchers also found that even when circumstances were most
conducive to charge reductions, other factors (such as prosecutor office
policies or drug weight) outside the code could reduce the likelihood
of charge reductions—as was the case with the cocaine cases they
examined.
128
In addition, when many charging options existed across
the felony-misdemeanor dividing line, the researchers found that more
available felony options made it less likely that felony charges would be
reduced to a misdemeanor, resulting in a higher percentage of charge
reductions that still yielded a felony conviction.
129
is was the case for
both serious and less serious felonies—a result the researchers confirmed
was independent of the seriousness of the original felony charge filed.
130
The substance of particular criminal oenses
It is not only the architecture of the criminal code and its sentencing
provisions that are influential in plea bargaining, but, as a 2012 descriptive
study found, also the specific content of a particular charged offense.
34 Vera Institute of Justice
Aer examining more than 620,000 criminal cases that terminated in
federal district court from 2002 to 2009, researchers showed that a crimes
“essential elements, the proof required to establish the offense, and the
basic gravamen of the crime” affect the frequency and composition of plea
bargains in those cases.
131
Charges that were secondary to the crux of the prosecutions case
(such as witness tampering) or that threatened added punishment
but remained outside the core prosecuted offense (such as use of a
firearm in the commission of a federal felony) had high dismissal
rates pursuant to plea agreements.
132
Both of these pes of charges
oen presume the existence of, and are related to, other oen
more serious crimes. Prosecutors may be willing to discard these
charges in order to facilitate a guil plea because they are viewed as
superfluous to the central allegations in the case.
133
Crimes that were easiest to prove (such as escape, reentry of a
deported person, failure to appear, or misprision—deliberate
concealment—of a felony) had the lowest frequency of charge
bargains, but high rates of guil pleas, suesting that people likely
accepted plea deals with lile bargaining involved.
134
Some of these
charges are also common enough (such as reentry of a deported
person, failure to appear, misprision of a felony) that they are easily
“commoditized” such that parties will likely recognize and agree to
the understood “going-rate” for a guil plea in that particular case.
135
Prosecutors in certain higher-stakes cases (such as deprivation
of civil rights) frequently went to trial, despite a high likelihood
of acquial.
136
is was in part because sentencing cliffs between
charging options (for example, within the different gradations of
aravated assault categories) made charge reductions pursuant to
a plea deal difficult.
137
In other instances, political and institutional
pressures, combined with few aractive plea options, likely drove
cases to trial despite the difficul prosecutors faced in proving
their case.
138
Similarly, people were more likely to prefer trial despite
low acquial rates in other pes of cases (such as tax evasion or
possession of child pornography) either out of a belief they held in
principle (such as “federal taxation is unconstitutional”) or because
the perceived costs of conviction (such as stigma of sex offenses)
related to the alleged offense were deemed too high to agree to a
In the Shadows: A Review of the Research on Plea Bargaining 35
plea deal.
139
ese findings demonstrate that the likelihood of a plea
deal is not always driven by parties’ perceptions of the odds of a
particular trial outcome, but sometimes on a risk-benefit calculus.
Findings related to specific offenses argue against trying to generalize
plea bargaining behavior and practice across broad categories of charges
such as “proper offenses” or “violent offenses.” Doing so may hide
significant variations between specific charges within each offense category
(for example, under violent offenses, the distinction between simple assault
and murder). is, in turn, risks masking variations in offense-specific plea
bargaining behavior and outcomes in practice. is suests more broadly
that any effort to reform plea bargaining practices and outcomes must
incorporate legal reform, both substantive and procedural.
Caseloads
Beyond the structure of the criminal code, other system-level elements
factor into the use of plea bargaining. ere has long been a common
assumption that the high use of plea bargaining in U.S. criminal courts is,
at least in part, a result of the high caseloads many courts, prosecutors, and
defense counsel must process.
140
Indeed, it appears undeniable that if even
a fraction of the people who currently plead guil were to instead demand
their right to a trial, courts and their staff would quickly be overwhelmed.
e continued nctioning of the U.S. justice system is dependent on the
high number of guil pleas that are entered every year.
141
With this in mind, a small body of research has sought to understand
how variations in caseloads might influence individual cases. Researchers
have asked to what extent plea deals are influenced by the case volumes
of the specific court or prosecutor to which the case is assigned. (Research
has tended to ignore the caseload pressures faced by defense counsel.)
Studies conducted during the past 40 years have found lile evidence of
a significant relationship.
142
Observational and interview-based research
published almost half a century ago in 1978 compared several low- and
high-volume criminal courts and found no noticeable differences in the
ways in which prosecutors approached and applied plea bargaining to
their cases.
143
Quantitative analysis of courts in Chicago published around
36 Vera Institute of Justice
Displaced sentencing discretion? The impact of sentencing guidelines on
charge bargaining practices
To help protect against the risk of judges relying on improp-
er factors in sentencing—such as race, gender, or personal
beliefs—and better ensure that similarly situated people receive
comparable sentences, at least 20 state jurisdictions, the District
of Columbia, and the federal government have promulgated
sentencing guidelines to both guide and curb judicial discretion
at sentencing.
a
Some jurisdictions have enacted presumptive
guidelines, meaning that judges are required to impose a
recommended sentence within the prescribed range or provide
compelling and substantial reasons for deviating from them.
b
Other jurisdictions put in place voluntary guidelines, which do
not require judges to impose a recommended sentence.
c
Even
within these two broad categories, there are wide variations in
the degree to which guidelines curb judicial discretion.
d
One enduring concern, however, is the extent to which sentenc-
ing guidelines shift the locus of sentencing discretion to prose-
cutors—and how this may perpetuate the very disparities that
sentencing guidelines were supposed to eliminate.
e
Under these
systems, prosecutors can achieve their preferred sentences (or
avoid those with which they disagree) through the manipulation
of the charges they pursue (by choosing the types and number
of charges filed, the types of charges they are willing to drop,
or the types of charge and/or sentence reductions they are
willing to oer), using this as leverage in plea negotiations. Five
studies that looked at the impact of sentencing guidelines on
plea bargaining practices are examined here, providing mixed
results on whether sentencing guidelines, or dierent types of
guidelines, increase charge bargaining among prosecutors.
In four of the five studies, researchers examined the eect of
sentencing guidelines enactment and found little or no evidence
that sentencing guidelines substantially altered prosecutorial
charge bargaining practices. For example, a 1987 study looking
at the eect of Minnesota’s presumptive sentencing guidelines
on prosecutors’ plea bargaining practices—comparing cas-
es from 1978 (pre-guidelines) and cases from 1981 and 1982
(post-guidelines)—found that prosecutorial charging practices
(average severity of the initial charge, rate of charge dismissal,
rate of charge reduction, and rate of sentence negotiation)
were largely the same before and after the guidelines went into
eect.
f
In particular, it found no proof during the post-guidelines
period that prosecutors had started overcharging cases in order
to better facilitate guilty pleas.
g
A 2005 study conducting a
comparable analysis of Ohio’s 1996 enactment of presumptive
sentencing guidelines came to a similar conclusion.
h
It found no
significant changes across most measures (the likelihoods of
indictment on a first- or second-degree felony, all charges being
dropped after indictment, pleading guilty with prosecutorial
agreement, and some dropped charges), although it did uncov-
er a modest 4 percent increase post-guidelines in the likelihood
of charge reductions among people who pled guilty.
i
Signifi-
cantly, it found that racial disparities persisted, even worsened,
after guidelines implementation—for example, finding that for
Black people, charges were less likely to be dropped under the
guidelines system than prior.
j
Two further pre-/post-guidelines studies using dierent meth-
odologies analyzed the implementation of Washington, DC’s
voluntary sentencing guidelines and found similar results.
k
In
2014, researchers found little statistically significant impact
on prosecutors’ charge bargaining practices (for example,
rate and extent of charge bargaining and extralegal eects on
charge bargaining outcomes) after guideline adoption, other
than a small observed increase in the number of felony charges
dismissed or reduced for initial charges in the most severe
charge category.
l
In 2019, after comparing dierences in the
value of charge bargains—as evidenced by the magnitude of
sentence reductions that flow from them—researchers again
found only minimal changes. Prior to sentencing guidelines,
people accepting charge-bargained pleas received more than
30 percent shorter sentences than if they had been sentenced
on their arraignment charge; after guidelines enactment, peo-
ple accepting charge-bargained pleas received sentences that
were nearly 36 percent shorter.
m
Although the small dierences observed in both Washington,
DC, studies suggest that prosecutors may have filed more
serious charges in certain cases in the post-guidelines period to
expedite the plea bargaining process, the relative stability of all
other outcomes between the pre- and post-guidelines periods in
both studies points to little interaction eect between sentenc-
ing guidelines and charge bargaining practices and outcomes.
n
For all four studies discussed above, one potential explanation
for the lack of change across the comparison periods may be
the relatively narrow time range under examination after guide-
lines were implemented. Indeed, looking only six months out
after guidelines implementation in Ohio—or one to two years
out after implementation in Washington, DC, and Minnesota, re-
spectively—may not have allowed enough time for practitioners
to fully understand and use the new rules.
o
(Another possible
explanation is that while prosecutors may not have changed
their charging practices after implementation of sentencing
guidelines, it is possible that they had already been overcharg-
ing before the guidelines were implemented.)
A fifth study examined whether the type of sentencing guide-
lines—presumptive or voluntary—makes a dierence to pros-
ecutors’ charge bargaining practices and resulting sentencing
outcomes. After comparing data from one county in one state
with presumptive guidelines (Washington State) with data
from two counties in a state with voluntary guidelines (Mary-
land), the 2007 study found that the county under more rigid
sentencing guidelines saw greater charge bargaining impacts,
demonstrated by larger observed reductions in sentence
lengths and in declines in the probability of prison sentences.
p
However, the study used a predictive model that tested what
would happen if Washington State cases were adjudicated in
Maryland, and vice versa. This potentially minimizes the extent
to which the researchs findings can be applied more generally,
given its hypothetical nature.
q
Future research should investi-
gate the appropriateness of the strong assumptions underlying
this approach.
In the Shadows: A Review of the Research on Plea Bargaining 37
the same time supported this conclusion.
144
More recent analyses found
that cases tried in courts with higher caseloads are subject to slightly
harsher trial penalties than those tried in lower-volume courts.
145
is may
indicate that charge and sentencing decisions are influenced, in part, by
considerations relating to organizational efficiency—where higher caseload
courts adjust their practices in order to see fewer cases go to trial.
146
However, research that has sought to measure the caseload pressures on
the people directly responsible for plea offers—the prosecutors assigned
to each case—has offered less support for this hypothesis, finding lile
relationship between case volume and the use of plea bargaining.
Two studies statistically analyzed drug cases in the office of the New
York Coun District Aorney that were disposed of in 2010 and 2011
one looking at approximately 1,200 felony drug cases and the other at a
similar number of misdemeanor marijuana cases.
147
While controlling for
multiple case and defendant characteristics, the studies considered the
active caseload size of the prosecutors assigned to each case.
148
e study
of felony cases found no relationship between the size of each prosecutor’s
If even a fraction of the people who
currently plead guilty were to instead
demand their right to a trial, courts and
their sta would quickly be overwhelmed.
The continued functioning of the U.S.
justice system is dependent on the high
number of guilty pleas that are entered
every year.
38 Vera Institute of Justice
caseload and the charge or sentence offers (the measures of plea bargaining)
made in their cases.
149
e researchers noted, however, that prosecutors
working in New York Ci may be accustomed to high case volumes and
that the results may not be generalizable to jurisdictions that traditionally
process fewer cases.
150
e analysis of misdemeanor marijuana cases
similarly revealed that caseload pressure was unrelated to the charge offers
made, but the study did find that the likelihood of being offered a plea deal
that included a custodial sentence increased marginally as prosecutors
caseloads increased.
151
Another study of 318,750 felony and misdemeanor cases filed in
Wisconsin from 2009 to 2013 similarly analyzed the relationship between
prosecutor characteristics—including the size and make-up of their
caseloads—and case outcomes, while controlling for numerous defendant
and case variables.
152
e researchers found a large range in the number
of cases the prosecutors worked on—from fewer than 100 in a five-year
period to several thousand.
153
However, there was no relationship between
the size of prosecutors’ caseloads and either their case dismissal rate or the
plea bargain outcomes of their cases (in other words, the likelihood of guil
pleas to lesser charges or of pleas resulting in noncustodial sentences).
154
Although there was no evidence that caseload size influenced
prosecutors’ plea bargaining behavior, the composition of prosecutors
caseloads was found to be somewhat predictive of case outcomes. e
same study of cases in Wisconsin found that prosecutors with a higher
proportion of violent cases in their caseloads were more likely to dismiss
cases and more likely to agree to a guil plea to a lesser charge.
155
ey
were, however, less likely to accept a noncustodial sentence than other
prosecutors; the researchers hypothesize that this may reflect a greater
willingness to engage in charge bargaining, but less willingness to engage
in sentence bargaining among these prosecutors.
156
Conversely, prosecutors with high felony caseloads were less likely
to dismiss a case and less likely to agree to a plea to a lesser charge than
prosecutors with lower felony caseloads.
157
e researchers propose that
prosecutors who handle more felony cases may come to view all cases
more seriously and place greater value on securing a more serious criminal
record.
158
It is notable that the findings of this study led the researchers
to conclude that prosecutors’ caseloads do not incentivize their plea
bargaining behavior with the motivation of “clearing their plates” or as
In the Shadows: A Review of the Research on Plea Bargaining 39
a concession to limited resources, but rather that the pe of cases that
prosecutors routinely try will influence how they evaluate plea bargaining
in all of their cases.
159
Although the research presented here suests that variation in
prosecutorial caseload size has lile notable impact on plea bargaining
at the case level, cases are not processed in a vacuum, and differences in
the working sles of prosecutors—and in the situational contexts of the
district aorney offices they work in—are likely to be influential.
160
Mixed
methods research combining focus groups, surveys, and quantitative
analysis of cases finds that, despite puing great emphasis on the value of
consistency in practice, prosecutors employ a wide range of sles in plea
bargaining, meaning plea offers can differ markedly simply as a nction of
the prosecutor assigned to the case.
161
However, earlier research conducted
in the 1980s suests that this variation can be constrained, to a degree, by
the level of control the local district aorney places on the plea bargaining
practices of their staff.
162
Plea bargaining outcomes
G
iven the centrali of guil pleas to the day-to-day nctioning of
the courts and to the vast majori of peoples experiences of the
justice system, it is important to understand the impact that plea
bargaining has on case outcomes. Specifically, researchers have aempted
to discern just how much of a bargain people may receive when they plead
guil (or, conversely, how much worse their punishment may be if they go
to trial). A related question also arises: are the benefits of plea bargaining
large enough to coerce innocent people to plead guil? And if so, how
oen does this happen? As described below, studies that have sought to
address these questions have, in the process, revealed how complex and
opaque the system truly is.
40 Vera Institute of Justice
How much of a bargain is a plea bargain?
e process of plea bargaining is predicated on the contention that
prosecutors can offer people the opportuni for more favorable outcomes
if they plead guil rather than take their cases to trial. (Whether this more
favorable outcome is best characterized as a “plea discount” or a “trial
penal” is the subject of some debate.)
163
However, the pical magnitude
of this dispari in punishment is uncertain. Some researchers have found
evidence of a large difference in sentencing outcomes.
164
Others have
suested that the difference may be minimal (or even, as in one study,
nonexistent)—although, as discussed below, these studies have been
critiqued on methodological and conceptual bases.
165
One study does provide evidence of a substantial trial penal.
Researchers looked at serious violent felonies resolved in Pennsylvania
trial courts in the late 1990s using regression models to measure the
relationship between plea bargaining and sentencing while accounting
for other important factors (such as the severi of the offense and the
defendant’s characteristics).
166
ey found that the odds of incarceration
were 2.7 times greater for people tried by a jury than for those who pled
guil.
167
In addition, sentence lengths were 57 percent longer for people
convicted by trial jury compared to those who pled guil.
168
The trial penalty.
64% longer
custodial sentences
imposed at trial
Length of custodial sentences
Sentences
imposed
at trial
Sentences
imposed
through
pleas
In the Shadows: A Review of the Research on Plea Bargaining 41
Research has found, however, considerable variation in the magnitude
of plea discounts both between and within jurisdictions.
169
is is perhaps
unsurprising given the varying levels of constraint in the use of plea
bargaining that prosecutors work under in different states: jurisdictions
vary in the use of, and adherence to, plea bargaining guidelines, and
district aorneys maintain varying levels of oversight and control over
assistant aorneys’ plea deal offers.
170
One analysis of sentencing data
from five states from 1997 to 2004 used regression analyses to compare
the sentence lengths received following pleas, bench trials, and jury trials,
while controlling for case and defendant characteristics.
171
In each state
and for most offenses, sentences received aer jury trial were significantly
longer than those received following a plea; there was, however, notable
variation by jurisdiction and offense.
172
In Maryland, at one end of the
spectrum, jury trials for heroin distribution cases resulted in incarceration
sentences that were 350 percent longer than those given following a plea.
173
Similarly, cocaine distribution cases disposed of by jury trial were seven
times more likely to result in incarceration than those seled through a
plea.
174
In Washington State, however, there was no significant difference
in sentence length among cases resolved through plea, bench trial, or jury
trial for five of the 12 offenses studied.
175
Still, despite the variation, the
researchers concluded that “judges and prosecutors are imposing more
lenient sentences for defendants who plead guil.”
176
is is, in essence, the
“bargain” underlying the entire plea bargaining process.
In federal criminal courts, plea discounts are baked into the system,
with sentencing guidelines allowing for sentence reductions when a
person “accepts responsibili” for the alleged crime—a condition that a
guil plea can satis.
177
A study looking at 207,000 federal cases from
2006 to 2008 demonstrates the potential impact of these guidelines.
178
e
study used regression analysis to control for a range of case and defendant
characteristics, including offense severi, and found that the average
custodial sentence imposed at trial was 64 percent longer than sentences
reached through pleas.
179
As in state courts, the researchers found
substantial variation by crime pe—with firearms trial sentences being 29
percent longer than pled sentences, while larceny and the offenses were
associated with a 137 percent trial penal.
180
A similar study of 115,000 federal convictions from 2000 to 2002 (in
which 5 percent of cases were resolved at trial, and 95 percent through
42 Vera Institute of Justice
a guil plea) found that, aer accounting for various case and defendant
characteristics, including offense severi, incarceration sentence lengths
were 45 percent longer for people convicted at trial than those convicted
through a plea.
181
(e slightly lower trial penal detected here may be
because this study excluded cases that resulted in a probation sentence,
whereas the previous study included these as zero-length sentences
of incarceration).
182
However, trial penalties were not solely a product
of sentencing guidelines; aer guideline-based sentencing changes
were controlled for, people convicted at trial were still found to receive
sentences 16 percent longer than people who pled guil.
183
Two recent studies have aempted to cast doubt on the potential
“benefits” of pleading guil (such as reduced sentences), both claiming
that people may, on average, experience beer outcomes by choosing to
go to trial due to the possibili of acquial.
184
e first, a study of more
than 91,000 misdemeanor cases accepted for prosecution in 2010 and 2011
in New York Ci, partially corroborated the results of previous research,
finding that people convicted at trial were more likely to be incarcerated,
faced longer probation sentences, and received larger fines than people
who pled guil—and that these differences remained aer controlling
for case and defendant characteristics.
185
(e researchers were unable
to control for the effects of pretrial detention on case outcomes due to
missing data.)
186
However, the study also found that two cases out of every
ve that went to trial resulted in acquials—a far higher acquial rate than
expected.
187
is, the researchers concluded, suests that more people may
benefit from taking a case to trial than currently do so.
188
ere are, however, significant caveats to this conclusion. Importantly,
fewer than 0.5 percent of cases in the study’s sample actually went to
trial—and those that resulted in conviction, on average, had more severe
sentences than cases in which a person pled guil.
189
With such a tiny
fraction of cases going to trial, it is possible that these are exceptional cases
that differ from the average case in unmeasured ways—in fact, the study
found that people represented by private aorneys, who presumably had
more resources and who may have had cases deemed more ‘winnable,
were more likely to go to trial—and that the high acquial rate would
not be sustained should more people choose not to plead guil.
190
e
researchers also do not consider the length of time people may spend in jail
awaiting an eventual acquial and the harms that such pretrial detention
In the Shadows: A Review of the Research on Plea Bargaining 43
precipitates. However, the finding that 99.6 percent of these misdemeanor
cases were resolved through a guil plea suests that the plea bargaining
process is substantially different for misdemeanor cases than it is for
felonies.
191
(See “Misdemeanor justice and plea bargains” at page 16.)
e second study, a contentious examination of more than 40,000
felony cases resolved before 2004 in Cook Coun (Chicago), Illinois,
went rther by concluding that not only is there no real trial penal,
but there is, in fact, a “trial discount.”
192
e researcher found that, if all
sentences were averaged with acquials counted as “zero-year” sentence
lengths, trials resulted in sentences 14 months shorter than guil pleas.
193
(However, when acquials were not counted, people who were convicted at
trial were found, as in other studies, to be more likely to receive a custodial
sentence and to receive longer sentences than those who pled guil.)
194
e results and methodology of this study have been heavily criticized,
however. e analysis assumes that people who pled guil would have the
same chances of acquial as those who went to trial—which is unlikely
to be true.
195
Furthermore, the study categorizes a large number of cases
as acquials that were, in fact, other forms of case termination—such as
cases that were terminated when a person failed to return to court, cases
transferred to other jurisdictions, or cases transferred to immigration
enforcement—thus substantially inflating the likelihood of “acquial” even
though the people involved were likely to have been convicted later or in
another court.
196
Finally, the study does not consider people who were held
in pretrial detention but then acquied at trial to have been incarcerated,
despite the fact that they may have experienced a significant amount of
time in custody.
e studies reviewed here demonstrate that the magnitude of the
average plea discount, or trial penal, differs between jurisdictions
and offenses. Furthermore, the methodological and conceptual choices
researchers make in studying sentencing outcomes can also create large
disparities in findings. Despite this disagreement, there is a consensus that,
if a person is to be convicted and sentenced, it will be beer for them if
this happens following a plea, not a trial.
197
44 Vera Institute of Justice
Are innocent people induced to
plead guilty?
e incentives offered through plea bargaining, or the penalties associated
with going to trial, may be substantial enough to induce people who are
innocent of a crime to plead guil.
198
is is perhaps especially true for
lower-level cases in which a guil plea may hasten case disposal and
secure a persons release from custody. (See “Misdemeanor justice and plea
bargains” at page 16.) Innocent people charged with more serious crimes
may still be induced to plead guil, either to avoid a harsher punishment
at conviction or, if they are already incarcerated but likely to win their case
(and exoneration) on appeal, to secure an immediate release from prison on
a sentence of time served rather than wait out a lengthy appeal process.
199
Estimating the frequency with which this happens is, however,
challenging; researchers have primarily approached the question using
defendant interviews and analyses of exonerations, both of which, as
described below, come with significant limitations. Such challenges
mean that lile research has been conducted specifically on guil pleas.
Research into false admissions of guilt has tended to focus more on false
confessions to the police—and the coercive tactics used in interrogation
and less on false guil pleas made to prosecutors or the conditions that
make them more likely.
200
Researchers hypothesize that false guil pleas
may be more common than false confessions because promises of leniency
(via reduced charges or at sentencing), which may be very compelling to
people, are not permied during police interrogations.
201
Despite these
limitations, research does appear to confirm that, among the millions
of cases that are seled through guil pleas each year, a meaningl
number of people are actually innocent of the crimes with which they
were charged. For example, in a 2018 study of 166 aorneys, 148 of the
participants said that they had been involved in at least one case in which
their client chose to plead guil despite maintaining their innocence.
202
Self-reported innocence
Interviews with justice-involved people suest that false guil pleas may
be disturbingly common—at least among people who might be considered
In the Shadows: A Review of the Research on Plea Bargaining 45
especially vulnerable, such as young people or those with mental illnesses.
A survey of more than 1,200 people with mental illnesses who were
currently in jail or facing criminal charges in court found that more than
one-third—37 percent—reported having falsely pled guil at some point
in their lives.
203
In making these false guil pleas, the majori stated
that they were motivated to do so by the desire “to end the questioning,
get [out] of jail, or go home.”
204
In an interview study of nearly 200
incarcerated boys between the ages of 14 and 17 years old, nearly one-fih
of participants reported having made false guil pleas during their lives.
205
In 51 percent of these cases, the young people reported that they had
made a false guil plea in order to avoid more severe consequences and
lessen the punishment they received.
206
e likelihood that a young person
reported a false guil plea increased with the number of high pressure
tactics they said they had been exposed to by lawyers (such as threats of
greater punishment or the use of deception).
207
Exonerations
Another way of studying innocence among people who plead guil is to
look at cases that were subsequently overturned in court—oen through
the discovery of new evidence, such as DNA. Exonerations following guil
pleas are not common for several reasons: first, guil pleas are oen given
in exchange for a reduced sentence or release from jail, diminishing the
perceived urgency or necessi of proving someones factual innocence;
second, if someone has pled guil, it is more difficult to convince others
of their innocence or that it is appropriate to invest the resources needed
to investigate and secure an exoneration; and third, by pleading guil,
people may lose access to mechanisms of appeal and due process that
would facilitate the exoneration process.
208
Nevertheless, a small number of
studies have shown that innocent people can be compelled to plead guil.
One study looked at 466 felony convictions, spanning from 1989
through 2011, in which the accused were later proven to be factually
innocent and were exonerated.
209
e researchers found that in
approximately 8 percent of these cases, the person was convicted following
a guil plea.
210
Similarly, research conducted by the Innocence Project
into 362 convictions that were overturned through DNA evidence found
46 Vera Institute of Justice
that people in 11 percent of these cases had pled guil.
211
Researchers in
studies such as these are unable to estimate the frequency of false guil
pleas more generally because the sample they are drawn from—cases
overturned through exoneration—do not reflect the pical case. ey are
also limited in their generalizabili because they focus on felony cases,
with the majori of convictions in the samples being for homicide and
sex offenses.
212
e results of these studies do not, therefore, indicate the
frequency of false guil pleas in lower-level felony or misdemeanor cases.
Indeed, exonerations for lower-level cases—in which exculpatory scientific
evidence might not exist and in which people are less motivated to spend
time and resources proving someones innocence—almost never happen.
213
Academics theorize, however, that it is for lower-level charges, where
the stakes of pleading guil are lower and the benefits of a faster case
resolution are higher, that false guil pleas may be most common.
214
Much of what has been learned through exonerations about innocent
people being compelled to plead guil to misdemeanors comes from
Harris Coun (Houston), Texas.
215
Indeed, while only 4 percent of
exonerations in the United States from 1989 to 2011 were for misdemeanor
cases (85 out of 2,145 cases), 67 percent of these cases come from Harris
Coun.
216
Unlike most other parts of the country, Harris Coun operates
Research shows what is known
intuitively—that cases exist in which the
plea oer made, and the opportunity to
be released from jail, eectively coerce
people who are factually innocent into
pleading guilty.
In the Shadows: A Review of the Research on Plea Bargaining 47
a drug lab that continues to test substances collected by the police even
aer the case has concluded.
217
e coun has uncovered hundreds of
felony and misdemeanor cases in which innocent people pled guil to
drug possession.
218
A review of the misdemeanor cases found that the
majori of people pled guil in order to be released from jail as they
were otherwise unable to afford bail; pleading not guil would likely have
resulted in several more months in jail, with the prospect of years in prison
if found guil at trial.
219
(See “Misdemeanor justice and plea bargains,” on
page 16.) Indeed, researchers have hypothesized that, at least for lower-level
charges, factually innocent people may feel the greatest pressure to plead
guil as it is to these people that prosecutors, faced with lile compelling
evidence of their guilt, will make their most generous plea offers in order
to secure a conviction.
220
Ultimately, research shows what is known intuitively—that cases exist
in which the plea offer made, and the opportuni to be released from jail,
effectively coerce people who are factually innocent into pleading guil.
e scale of this problem, however, remains difficult to determine.
221
Harris County, Texas, operates a drug lab
that continues to test substances collected by
police even after a case has been concluded.
The county has uncovered hundreds of felony
and misdemeanor cases in which innocent
people pled guilty to drug possession.
False guilty pleas in Harris County, Texas.
48 Vera Institute of Justice
Conclusion: Limitations and
ture directions
A
dmiedly, the dynamic and iterative nature of plea bargaining—a
largely undocumented process that permeates the entire life of
a criminal case, long or short—presents significant challenges to
researchers trying to identi and isolate relationships between key variables
and plea bargaining practices or outcomes. Plea bargaining neither occurs
at a single moment in time, nor is it a definitive quantifiable product that
remains constant. Rather, it is a complex, interactive dialogue involving
multiple people and various considerations, many of which are beyond
the purview of current criminal justice data collection capabilities (such as
transaction costs, financial access, or psychological and cognitive biases) and,
therefore, remain largely untested and unaccounted for in current research.
As a result, pathways to a negotiated plea deal are not likely
straightforward, nor as easily predictable as some research, given limited
datasets, may suest. For example, reducing a persons choice whether
to accept a plea offer to a single factor—the hypothetical case outcome
if the case were to go to trial—is perhaps too simplistic a frame. It not
only ignores the inherently coercive nature of pretrial detention, but also
structural factors (poor lawyering, agency costs, and lawyers’ self-interest)
and individual psychological or behavioral ones (such as overconfidence,
self-serving biases, denial mechanisms, and risk preference) that shape
peoples actions and motives in ways that can heavily influence plea
bargaining processes, behavior, and outcomes.
222
Given this complex
nature, it is understandable why existing studies fail to satisfactorily
describe, explain, or prove the ll suite of factors that influence plea
bargaining behavior and outcomes—and why definitive answers are
difficult to discern.
223
e result is a mix of complicated, nuanced, and sometimes
contradictory research findings. For example, when researchers have
compared bargained-for sentences and sentences that would have resulted
from trial, only some researchers found evidence of a large difference.
In the Shadows: A Review of the Research on Plea Bargaining 49
Nor, evidently, has it been easy for researchers to estimate the true scale
of innocent people induced to plead guil, as data and sampling issues
limit studies to only those cases in which people had been exonerated
the tiniest subset of total cases and likely not representative of the pical
American criminal case. Although these mixed results are likely due to
differences in methodological approaches and sample pes, it would be
unwise to underestimate the inherent difficul of puing together the
various pieces of the “plea-bargaining puzzle.”
224
And only a handl of
researchers have managed to design studies that aempted to capture
the motivations and factors influencing the people pleading guil or the
bargaining power and resources of their defense counsel.
Ultimately, the lack of clari regarding the administration and impacts
of plea bargaining is perhaps the most important thing that research
has revealed. Guil pleas account for the vast majori of criminal case
outcomes and, as such, form the bedrock of the U.S. criminal legal system.
at plea bargaining should remain so obscured, that its biases and
injustices should prove so impervious to being seen and understood, is
beyond problematic. Fixing the major failings of Americas justice system—
including mass incarceration and systemic racism—is made exponentially
more difficult when the most common and most ndamental of court
operations is largely invisible. It is therefore incumbent upon court actors,
legislators, advocates, researchers, and the communi to demand a system
that embraces greater transparency. Only by recording plea bargaining
processes and decisions and by making these records available can U.S.
courts and prosecutors begin to be held accountable.
Only by recording plea bargaining
processes and decisions and by making
these records available can U.S. courts and
prosecutors begin to be held accountable.
50 Vera Institute of Justice
Box notes
“Law of plea bargaining: An overview” p. 8
a Mary Vogel, “The Social Origins of Plea Bargaining: Conflict and the
Law in the Process of State Formation, 1830-1860,” Law & Society
Review 33, no. 1 (1999), 161-246 (explaining that plea bargaining
came about as a legal innovation in response to local political
and social forces of the time—a time marked by a perceived rise
in crime, increased immigration and thus ethnic diversity, and the
erosion of social cohesion and cultural consensus); and Albert
Alschuler, “Plea Bargaining and Its History,” Columbia Law Review
79, no. 1 (1979), 24-32 (describing the growth of plea bargaining and
extent of its use in the early 20th century), https://perma.cc/
BK4N-ZFR9. See also Mark H. Haller, “Plea Bargaining: The
Nineteenth Century Context,” Law & Society Review 13, no. 2 (1979),
273-79, 273 (“[P]lea bargaining was probably nonexistent before
1800, began to appear during the early or mid-nineteenth century,
and became institutionalized as a standard feature of American
urban criminal courts in the last third of the nineteenth century”).
b Santobello v. New York, 404 U.S. 257, 261 (1971), https://perma.cc/
Z7RT-S9MT; and Missouri v. Frye, 566 U.S. 134 (2012), https://perma.
cc/5RAT-SCZ8. Also see Lafler v. Cooper, 566 U.S. 156 (2012) (“The
criminal justice system for the most part is a system of pleas, not a
system of trials”), https://perma.cc/WQH6-TETJ.
c Mabry v. Johnson, 467 U.S. 504, 508 (1984) (“Because each side
may obtain advantages when a guilty plea is exchanged for
sentencing concessions, the agreement is no less voluntary than
any other bargained-for exchange”), https://perma.cc/JS5J-BAPT;
and Brady v. United States, 397 U.S. 742, 752 (1970) (“It is this
mutuality of advantage that perhaps explains the fact that, at
present, well over three-fourths of the criminal convictions in this
country rest on pleas of guilty. . .”), https://perma.cc/D5R9-L4FQ.
For other cases where courts have used the contract analogy, see
Puckett v. United States, 556 U.S. 129, 136 (2009) (“Although the
analogy may not hold in all respects, plea bargains are essentially
contracts”), https://perma.cc/7MYG-FXXX/; and Frye, 566 U.S. at
8 (“Bargaining is, by its nature, defined to a substantial degree by
personal style . . . it may be neither prudent nor practicable to try to
elaborate or define detailed standards for the proper discharge of
defense counsel’s participation in the [plea bargaining] process.”).
d United States v. Mezzanatto, 513 U.S. 196, 206 (1995), https://
perma.cc/Z62E-PRJM.
e For example, while California defines plea bargaining by statute
as “any bargaining, negotiation, or discussion between a criminal
defendant, or his or her counsel, and a prosecuting attorney
or judge, whereby the defendant agrees to plead guilty or
nolo contendere, in exchange for any promises, commitments,
concessions, assurances, or consideration by the prosecuting
attorney or judge relating to any charge against the defendant or
to the sentencing of the defendant,” that section of the code does
not contain standards for those negotiations. Cal. Penal Code §
1192.7, https://perma.cc/VBG5-5CWA. The legislature later added
a separate law that “requires that a defendant’s guilty plea be
knowing, intelligent, and voluntary” and defines those terms. Cal.
Penal Code § 1016.8, https://perma.cc/GR2F-7YZA. Also see Wash.
Rev. Code § 9.94A.421, https://perma.cc/W39A-PLZF; and Wash.
Rev. Code § 9.94A.431 https://perma.cc/C8Y7-AU5Z. Other states
have chosen to promulgate administrative rules or court rules rather
than statutes to govern the process of plea bargaining. See for
example MD Rules, Rule 4-243, https://perma.cc/GS44-SZKN; Mass.
Rules of Criminal Procedure, Rule 12, https://perma.cc/3WML-
ACVU; and Ohio Rules of Criminal Procedure, Rule 11, https://
perma.cc/G9JZ-PBTQ.
f See Brady v. United States, 397 U.S. 742 (1970). By 1970, almost
90 percent of cases were resolved by guilty plea. Although data
collected in 20 states in 1978 suggests that guilty plea rates varied
between jurisdictions, it nevertheless confirms the wide use of guilty
pleas to resolve cases. See Herbert Miller, William McDonald, and
James Cramer, Plea Bargaining in the United States (Washington,
DC: National Institute of Law Enforcement and Criminal Justice,
Law Enforcement Assistance Administration, U.S. Department of
Justice, 1978), xii, https://perma.cc/2MYW-MPEJ. See also Herbert
Miller, William McDonald, and James Cramer, “Plea Bargaining
in the United States,” database (Ann Arbor, MI: Inter-university
Consortium for Political and Social Research, 1978), https://
perma.cc/S2GW-FZWJ.
g Brady, 397 U.S. at 748.
h Bordenkircher v. Hayes, 434 U.S. 357, 363 (1978) (“[I]n the ‘give-and-
take’ of plea bargaining, there is no such element of punishment
nor retaliation so long as the accused is free to accept or reject the
prosecution’s oer”), https://perma.cc/P5DW-KG8B.
i Brady, 397 U.S. at 750, 755; and Corbitt v. New Jersey, 439 U.S.
212, 218-219 (1978) (“[T]here is no per se rule against encouraging
guilty pleas. We have squarely held that a State may encourage
a guilty plea by oering substantial benefits in return for the
plea.”), https://perma.cc/5F73-N4HS. Also see Mezzanatto, 513
U.S. at 209-210 (1995) (“The plea bargaining process necessarily
exerts pressure on people to plead guilty and to abandon a series
of fundamental rights. . . .”). In addition to giving up their right
to trial by pleading guilty, people also give up accompanying
guarantees—including their right of confrontation and cross-
examination of witnesses, right to a jury, right against self-
incrimination, and the right only to be convicted on proof beyond
a reasonable doubt. See for example H.M. Emory, “The Guilty Plea
as a Waiver of Rights and as an Admission of Guilt,” Temple Law
Quarterly 44 (1970).
j United States v. Pollard, 959 F.2d 1011, 1021 (D.C. Cir. 1992), https://
perma.cc/UV77-F49X. However, guilty pleas may be voidable
(subject to annulment by a court at the request of a party) when
the prosecution has threatened physical harm, threatened to use
false testimony, or threatened additional prosecutions. Santobello,
In the Shadows: A Review of the Research on Plea Bargaining 51
404 U.S. at 265-66 (Douglas, J., concurring). For the definition of
voidable, see Steven H. Gifis, Law Dictionary (3d ed.) (New York:
Barron’s Educational Series, Inc., 1991), 521.
k In Santobello, the Court held that a prosecutor may not trick a
person into pleading guilty. It also held that, once a binding plea
agreement has been entered into, prosecutors may not renege
on their promises. Santobello, 404 U.S. at 266-267. In Santobello,
the original prosecutor for the case had agreed to make no
recommendation as to sentencing. However, by the time the plea
was entered, a new prosecutor had been assigned to the case
and recommended the maximum sentence. The judgment (and
sentence) was vacated and the case remanded to the state courts
to decide whether the guilty plea should be accepted on its original
terms (and the petitioner re-sentenced by a dierent judge) or
allowed to be withdrawn.
l See for example Corbitt, 439 U.S. at 218-219 (promise of leniency);
Brady, 397 U.S. at 752 (threatening the death penalty); and
Bordenkircher, 434 U.S. 357 (threatening to prosecute under a
dierent charge with a mandatory life sentence). See also United
States v. Goodwin, 457 U.S. 368, 380-383 (1982) (no presumption of
vindictiveness although harsher charges were not filed until after
accused claimed his right to jury trial), https://perma.cc/
95VP-6QLG; United States v. Usher, 703 F.2d 956, 958 (6th Cir. 1983)
(concluding that the person’s guilty plea was not coerced when he
was required to plead guilty in order for his wife to get a reduced
term), https://perma.cc/9D3E-AMCT; and Harman v. Mohn, 683 F.2d
834, 836-838 (4th Cir. 1982) (holding that the person’s guilty plea
was not coerced when the government agreed not to prosecute his
wife in exchange for a plea), https://perma.cc/QK9D-3WC5.
m Henderson v. Morgan, 426 U.S. 637, 647 (1976) (holding that a person
must understand the nature of the charges against him but need
not comprehend every element of the charged oense(s), only
those elements considered critical), https://perma.cc/T5UZ-4KAA;
Bousley v. United States, 523 U.S. 614, 618-619 (1998) (establishing
that if the accused is misinformed about the elements of the
oense, the plea is constitutionally invalid), https://perma.cc/
Y8LZ-T4KS; and Boykin v. Alabama, 395 U.S. 238, 243 & n.5 (1969),
(holding that people must understand the constitutional rights
waived by pleading guilty and that this understanding cannot be
presumed from silence), https://perma.cc/S6Z2-S9S4.
n See Fed. R. Crim. P. 11, https://perma.cc/P2X3-WJC6. For examples
of comparable state rules, see N.Y. Crim. Proc. Law § 220, https://
perma.cc/J3T7-8LQN; Tex. Crim. Pro. Code Ann. § 27, https://
perma.cc/YSN8-UCXG; and Cal. Penal Code § 1002-12. Also see
Julian Cook III, “Federal Guilty Pleas: Inequities, Indigence and the
Rule 11 Process,” Boston College Law Review, 60, no. 4 (2019), 1074
(discussing the plea colloquies in Lee v. United States and Class
v. United States). In general, the court also has the responsibility
not to enter judgment before determining that there is a factual
basis for the plea. However, the factual basis of a plea need not
be accurate or complete. See Fed. R. Crim. P. 11. The evidentiary
support for a plea’s factual basis can come from a variety of
sources—from the prosecution’s recitation of facts, a pre-sentence
report, witnesses, admissions from defendants, or even preliminary
hearings. See Brandon Garrett, “Why Plea Bargains Are Not
Confessions,” William & Mary Law Review 57, no. 4 (2016), 1415,
1428-1431, https://perma.cc/SZS6-TXUE. People can also plead
guilty while simultaneously protesting their innocence—these guilty
pleas are known as “Alford pleas,” after the case of North Carolina
v. Alford, 400 U.S. 25, 36 n.8 (1970) (accused agreed to plead
guilty and accept the maximum sentence that could be imposed
judicially—a term of 30 years’ imprisonment—rather than face a
jury trial, which had the option of imposing a death sentence; the
court held that this was a valid choice among his options and that
the plea was valid despite containing a statement that the person
had not committed the crime to which he pled guilty), https://
perma.cc/DH8V-BET8.
o Cook, “Federal Guilty Pleas,” (2019), 1074, 1107 and 1109 (discussing
the plea colloquies in Lee v. United States and Class v. United
States). Also see Stephanos Bibas, “Designing Plea Bargaining
from the Ground Up: Accuracy and Fairness Without Trials as
Backstops,” William & Mary Law Review 57, no. 4 (2016), 1055-
1081, 1059, 1065, 1074. Silence has been held to be inadequate to
waive constitutional rights. Boykin, 395 U.S. at 243 & n.5 (holding
that people must understand the constitutional rights waived by
pleading guilty and that this understanding cannot be presumed
from silence). However, only a brief armative is required under
United States v. Gardner, 417 F.3d 541 (6th Cir. 2005) (rejecting
the defendant’s claim that the mode of inquiry employed by the
court during the Rule 11 hearing—which “required only a yes-or-no
answer” to most questions—did not satisfy the “core concerns”
of Rule 11), https://perma.cc/9HZA-GLWK. The court reiterated its
ruling in United States v. Walker that, “[T]here is no requirement that
in order to rely on a defendant’s answer in a guilty-plea colloquy
to conclude that the defendant pleaded guilty knowingly and
voluntarily, those answers must be lengthy and all-encompassing; a
straightforward and simple ‘Yes, your Honor’ is sucient to bind a
defendant to its consequences.” 160 F.3d 1078, 1096 (6th Cir. 1998),
https://perma.cc/2YBK-VRGR.
p But see Burdick v. Quarterman, 504 F.3d 545, 547 (5th Cir. 2007)
(observing that due process requires a person’s understanding of
the maximum punishment that could be imposed), https://
perma.cc/ECR7-PP6S. The lack of clarity as to the sentence to be
imposed led the Second Circuit to conclude more recently that
a guilty plea failed to meet the “knowing” standard as well. See
United States v. Johnson, No. 15-3498-cr (2d. Cir. 2017), https://
perma.cc/MM4N-T5KQ. In Padilla v. Kentucky, the Supreme Court
explicitly extended the scope of the understanding required to the
collateral consequences of the plea. Padilla v. Kentucky, 559 U.S.
356 (2010) (petitioner was entitled to be advised that a conviction
was likely to result in his deportation before pleading guilty),
https://perma.cc/8B37-BVE3.
q Weatherford v. Bursey, 429 U.S. 545, 559 (1977), https://
supreme.justia.com/cases/federal/us/429/545/.
52 Vera Institute of Justice
r See United States v. Ruiz, 536 U.S. 622, 629-633 (2002) (rejecting
a constitutional right to impeachment or armative-defense
evidence in advance of a guilty plea), https://perma.cc/NAL9-BK5T.
Impeachment evidence raises questions about the credibility of a
witness while armative defense evidence is evidence that could
negate liability even if a crime was committed. Compare the fact
that civil litigants enjoy broad pretrial discovery from witness
depositions to interrogatories to document discovery. See Fed. R.
Civ. P. 26-37, https://perma.cc/TE29-VEAS. The holding in Ruiz seems
to run counter to that in Brady v. Maryland, 373 U.S. 83 (1963) (the
prosecution in a criminal trial has a duty to disclose evidence that
is favorable to the defense (exculpatory) and material to guilt or
sentencing), https://perma.cc/X8JP-AFZC.
For cases following Brady, see United States v. Agurs, 427 U.S. 97
(1976) (holding that disclosure of material described in Brady must
occur even absent specific request), https://perma.cc/D786-BAU7;
United States v. Bagley, 473 U.S. 667 (1985) (evidence is material
if “there is a reasonable probability that, had the evidence been
disclosed to the defense, the result of the proceeding would have
been dierent), https://perma.cc/E5GH-F35X; Giglio v. United
States, 405 U.S. 150 (1972) (exculpatory evidence includes “evidence
aecting” witness “credibility,” where the witness’ “reliability” is
likely “determinative of guilt or innocence”), https://perma.cc/
Z42S-XNGZ; and United States. v. Clark, No. 05-80810 (E.D. Mich.
2006) (recognizing that although there is no constitutional right to
broad discovery in a criminal case, limited disclosures are required
in criminal cases as a result of Brady), https://perma.cc/FDD5-ASUF.
It is unclear whether in Ruiz the Court meant to include all
material exculpatory evidence referenced in Brady. See McCann
v. Mangialardi, 337 F.3d 782, 788 (7th Cir. 2003) (reasoning that
Ruiz indicates a significant distinction between impeachment
information and exculpatory evidence” that makes it “highly likely
the Supreme Court would find a violation of the Due Process Clause
if prosecutors . . . have knowledge of a criminal defendant’s factual
innocence but fail to disclose such information to a defendant
before he enters into a guilty plea.”), https://perma.cc/74TW-9Z3G.
The Second and Fifth Circuits, on the other hand, have found that
Ruiz precludes all Brady challenges to guilty pleas. See Friedman
v. Rehal, 618 F.3d 142 (2d Cir. 2010), https://perma.cc/W4LM-X354;
and United States v. Conroy, 567 F.3d 174 (5th Cir. 2009), https://
perma.cc/QPW4-LRJW, respectively.
s Stephanos Bibas, “Regulating the Plea Bargaining Market: From
Caveat Emptor to Consumer Protection,” California Law Review 99,
no. 4 (2011), 1117-1161, https://perma.cc/HS2T-BLNG.
t These are: Padilla v. Kentucky, 559 U.S. 356 (2010), https://
perma.cc/M9QC-6Z6K; Missouri v. Frye, 566 U.S. 134 (2012), https://
perma.cc/2SXR-BW8L; Lafler v. Cooper, 566 U.S. 156 (2012), https://
perma.cc/59CN-MFDK; and Lee v. United States, 582 U.S. (2017),
https://perma.cc/AX9M-L56Y. Also see Hill v. Lockhart, 474 U.S. 52
(1985) (establishing that claims of ineective assistance of counsel
in the plea bargain context are governed by the two-part test set
forth in Strickland v. Washington, 466 U.S. 668 (1984)), https://
perma.cc/64UN-C3C7. However, there is a circuit split as to whether
this right to counsel applies during “informal” plea oers. In Frye,
the Court used the word “formal;” in Lafler, it did not. Some lower
courts are now applying the right to eective legal counsel only
to “formal” plea oers. See for example Johnson v. United States,
860 F. Supp. 2d 663, 789-90 (N.D. Iowa 2012) (establishing the
requirement of a formal plea oer from the prosecution to be a
bright line test governing whether a claim of prejudice arising from
counsel’s deficient performance in plea negotiations can be made),
https://perma.cc/K3VK-96HB; and DeFilippo v. United States,
No. 09-CV-4153 (NGG) (E.D.N.Y. Mar. 1, 2013) (“Thus, the lack of a
formal plea oer strongly weighs against a finding that [but for
the ineective assistance of counsel] DeFilippo would have pled
guilty”), https://perma.cc/4R3N-4PTG.
u “[A] valid guilty plea “forgoes not only a fair trial, but also other
accompanying constitutional guarantees. . . . includ[ing] the
privilege against compulsory self-incrimination, the jury trial
right, and the right to confront accusers. . . . A valid guilty plea
also renders irrelevant—and thereby prevents the defendant
from appealing—the constitutionality of case-related government
conduct that takes place before the plea is entered. Neither can
the defendant later complain that the indicting grand jury was
unconstitutionally selected.” Class v. United States, 583 U.S. ___
(2018) (citations omitted), https://perma.cc/77XF-R8XB. For trial
rights relinquished, see Boykin v. Alabama, 395 U.S. 238, 242-244
(1969) (a guilty plea involves relinquishing the rights to a jury
trial, to the assistance of trial counsel, to raise a defense, and to
confront accusers, and against self-incrimination). For antecedent
rights relinquished, see Tollett v. Henderson, 411 U.S. 258, 266 (1973)
(entry of a guilty plea forfeits an accused’s right to raise claims
of constitutional deprivation that occurred prior to the entry of
the guilty plea), https://perma.cc/S7GV-QU4H. For rights that are
not impliedly waived by a plea deal, see Menna v. New York, 423
U.S. 61, 63 & n.2 (1975) (holding that the accused did not waive his
double jeopardy claim when he entered the plea because his claim
was that the State could not convict him regardless of whether his
factual guilt was established), https://perma.cc/3SQH-3WKJ. Also
see Blackledge v. Perry, 417 U.S. 21 (1974), https://perma.cc/
Z98V-3MFC.
v For a discussion of express waiver provisions, see Alexandra W.
Reimelt, “An Unjust Bargain: Plea Bargains and Waiver of the Right
to Appeal,” Boston College Law Review 51, no. 3 (2010), 871, 876-879,
https://perma.cc/Y682-QYJ3. For rights that may not be waived,
see for example United States v. Mezzanatto, 513 U.S. 196, 204 (1995)
(suggesting that there are rights, such as the right to conflict-free
counsel, that are so integral to the fact-finding process that they
cannot be waived), https://perma.cc/LJ8F-3G7B.
w See Class v. United States, 583 U.S. (2018) (a guilty plea alone
does not bar people from challenging the constitutionality of the
statute under which they were convicted on direct appeal), https://
perma.cc/5S6S-EAUW; and Garza v. Idaho, 586 U.S. (2019)
In the Shadows: A Review of the Research on Plea Bargaining 53
(people have a constitutional right to an appeal even if they
have purportedly waived that right because some claims such as
whether the waiver is valid are unwaivable), https://perma.cc/
U9LV-XTM2.
x United States v. Lutchman, No. 17-291 (2d Cir. Dec. 6, 2018) (a
person’s appellate waiver in his plea agreement was not supported
by consideration and thus did not bar his challenge to his sentence
on appeal), https://perma.cc/J5DS-X2T4.
“Misdemeanor justice and plea bargains” p. 16
a Robert C. Boruchowitz, Malia N. Brink, and Maureen Dimino,
Minor Crimes, Massive Waste: The Terrible Toll of America’s Broken
Misdemeanor Courts (Washington, DC: National Association of
Criminal Defense Lawyers, 2009), 10-11 & 25-26, https://perma.cc/
U8RN-AV5T; and Jenny Roberts, “Why Misdemeanors Matter:
Defining Eective Advocacy in the Lower Criminal Courts,” UC
Davis Law Review 45 (2011), 277, 280-281, https://perma.cc/
W5JG-WMGV.
b Ian Weinstein, “The Adjudication of Minor Oenses in New York
City,” Fordham Urban Law Journal 31, no. 4 (2004), 1157, 1159-1160,
https://perma.cc/NNG3-ZM9S.
c Designations vary widely from jurisdiction to jurisdiction and
are largely a product of a particular jurisdiction’s labeling
choices. See Megan Stevenson and Sandra Mayson, “The Scale
of Misdemeanor Justice,” Boston University Law Review 98 (2018),
731, 739-740, https://perma.cc/2YSR-8NAZ. Also see Roberts, “Why
Misdemeanors Matter,” 2011, 277, 291-292; and Ian Weinstein, “The
Adjudication of Minor Oenses,” 2004, 1157, 1164-1166 (discussing
the wide breadth of minor criminal statutes in New York).
d Ram Subramanian, Ruth Delaney, Stephen Roberts et al.,
Incarceration’s Front Door: The Misuse of Jails in America (New
York: Vera Institute of Justice, 2015), https://perma.cc/K6UV-F45J.
Also see John Irwin, The Jail: Managing the Underclass in American
Society (Berkeley, CA: University of California Press, 1985), 2
(describing jail as an institution of control meant to “manage
society’s rabble”).
e Stevenson and Mayson, “The Scale of Misdemeanor Justice,” 2018,
731, 737. The authors note that between 2009 and the publication
of their study most people used a national estimate calculated by
the National Association of Criminal Defense Lawyers (NACDL)
in a 2009 study about misdemeanors. Extrapolating from 2006
data from 12 states, NACDL estimated that there were 10.5 million
misdemeanor cases nationally. Stevenson and Mayson suggest that
the discrepancy between their number and NACDL’s estimate may
be due to the fact that the 12 states used in the 2009 analysis could
have had misdemeanor filing rates lower than the national average,
creating a downward bias to the estimate. Ibid., 733 and 763-764.
For NACDLs estimate, see Boruchowitz, Brink, and Dimino, Minor
Crimes, Massive Waste, 2009, 11. For the statewide breakdown, see
National Center for State Courts, “Statewide Criminal Caseload
Composition in 31 States, 2016,” https://perma.cc/8AEX-SPVY. This
number has not changed substantially over the last decade. See
Robert LaFountain, Richard Schauer, Shauna Strickland et al.,
Examining the Work of State Courts: An Analysis of 2008 State
Court Caseloads (Washington, DC: National Center for State
Courts, 2010), 24 (finding that in 11 state courts, 79 percent of all
cases in which a district attorney filed charges involved people
accused of misdemeanor crimes), https://perma.cc/92VU-2KLK.
Also see Preeti Chauhan, Adam G. Fera, Megan B. Welsh et al.,
Trends in Misdemeanor Arrests in New York (New York: John Jay
College of Criminal Justice, 2018), 16 (reporting that in New York
City, misdemeanor arrests numbered 179,427, and 271,205 criminal
summonses were issued in 2016), https://perma.cc/MA8M-FUDX.
f Compare Malcolm Feeley, The Process is Punishment (New York:
Russell Sage Foundation, 1979), 5 (stating that 80 to 90 percent
of all criminal prosecutions begin and end in the lower courts);
Josh Bowers, “Legal Guilt, Normative Innocence, and the Equitable
Decision Not to Prosecute,” Columbia Law Review 110, no. 7 (2010),
1655, 1717 and table 3 (demonstrating that in 2008, misdemeanors
in Iowa outnumbered felonies five to one [126,246 to 25,077]);
and Victor Eugene Flango, “Judicial Roles for Modern Courts,”
National Center for State Courts, 2013, https://perma.cc/
9XFM-63AE (noting that “approximately 80 percent of criminal
cases are misdemeanors, and more than 70 percent of them are
handled by municipal judges, justices of the peace, or magistrates
in limited-jurisdiction courts”). Also see Stevenson and Mayson, “The
Scale of Misdemeanor Justice,” 2018, 746-747, (reporting that NCSC
confirmed that the three-to-one ratio between misdemeanor and
felony caseloads remained stable between 2007 and 2016).
g Alexandra Natapo, “Misdemeanors,” Annual Review of Law
and Social Science 11 (2015), 255, 256 (stating that “the lowly
misdemeanor—not homicide or rape—is the paradigmatic
American crime and the paradigmatic product of the American
criminal system”). Most misdemeanor cases—95 percent—are
resolved by guilty plea. Ibid., 255-267, 259. This is also borne out
when looking at specific jurisdictions. For example, in 2014, only 0.2
percent of misdemeanor cases in New York City went to trial. See
Michael Rempel, Ashmini Kerodal, Joseph Spadafore, and Chris
Mai, Jail in New York City: Evidence-Based Opportunities for Reform
(New York: Center for Court Innovation and Vera Institute of Justice,
2017), 70, Table 5.5, https://perma.cc/JTY9-CRAC.
h See for example Issa Kohler-Hausmann, “Managerial Justice and
Mass Misdemeanors,” Stanford Law Review 66, no. 3 (2014), 611,
624, n.27 (arguing that misdemeanor justice in New York City
is not particularly interested in evidence or guilt, but rather in
exercising social control through marking, procedural hassle, and
performance), https://perma.cc/ND67-MAWV.
i Alexandra Natapo, “Aggregation and Urban Misdemeanors,”
Fordham Urban Law Journal 40, no. 3 (2016), 1043, 1070-1071,
https://perma.cc/Q3NU-JGCY. Also see Issa Kohler-Hausmann,
Misdemeanorland: Criminal Courts and Social Control in an Age of
Broken Windows Policing (Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press,
2018), 125 (noting that two DAs oces had “standard oers printed
54 Vera Institute of Justice
on a sheet that listed the oce policy for arraignment oers by
charge type and number of prior arrests”) and 656-657 (discussion
about standard deals).
j Alisa Smith and Sean Maddan, Three-Minute Justice: Haste and
Waste in Florida’s Misdemeanor Courts, (Washington, DC: National
Association of Criminal Defense Lawyers, 2011), 14-15 (of 1,649
misdemeanor adjudications in 21 Florida counties, “[a]lmost 70
percent of defendants observed entered a guilty or no contest plea
at arraignment”), https://perma.cc/883L-FSDW. In New York City,
more than 57 percent of all misdemeanor and violation cases reach
a disposition at arraignment. See Kohler-Hausmann, “Managerial
Justice,” (2014), 611, 654. Also see Boruchowitz, Brink, and Dimino,
Minor Crimes, Massive Waste, 2009, 31-32 (discussing how in many
jurisdictions defense lawyers are expected to “meet and plead”
their clients in order to resolve cases as soon as first appearance,
with cases taking a total of five minutes in certain jurisdictions);
and Feeley, The Process is Punishment, 1979, 9-11 (“While a few
cases took up as much as a minute or two of the court’s time . . . the
overwhelming majority of cases took just a few seconds”).
k For examples of hard-to-refuse deals, see Kohler-Hausmann,
“Managerial Justice,” 2014, 645 (in New York City, “[t]he
largest misdemeanor disposition category, the adjournment in
contemplation of dismissal (ACD), is a conditional dismissal [and]
. . . [t]he largest conviction category resulting from misdemeanor
arrests is the noncriminal violation or infraction”). For both ACDs
and noncriminal convictions, cases are sealed after a certain
specified period. Ibid., 649 and 651. Also see Josh Bowers,
“Punishing the Innocent,” University of Pennsylvania Law Review
156, no. 5 (2008), 1117, 1144 (noting that in New York City in 1998, 52
percent of all misdemeanor charges that ended in conviction were
reduced to pleas to noncriminal violations and that for defendants
with no criminal convictions, the rate of reduction was 86
percent. Even for people with both prior felony and misdemeanor
convictions, the rate of that type of reduction was more than 25
percent. Also, more than 50 percent of all misdemeanor charges
that ended in conviction resulted in non-jail dispositions. Of jail
sentences, 57 percent were for sentences of time served.)
l Josh Bowers, “Legal Guilt, Normative Innocence, and the Equitable
Decision Not to Prosecute,” Columbia Law Review 110 (2010),
1655, 1701-1702 and 1709. Indeed, Bowers found that in both Iowa
and New York, prosecutors almost never declined to charge
misdemeanor oenses largely because they expected to procure
quick guilty pleas in these cases. Ibid., 1716-1720. Also see Ibid., 1709
(in which the author discusses how “disposable” cases are where
prosecutors’ initial decisions are “dispositive on the question of
whether the defendant will ultimately end up with some type of
conviction.”). Also see Stephanos Bibas, “Plea Bargaining Outside
the Shadow of Trial,” Harvard Law Review 117, no. 8 (2004), 2463,
2464-2465 (discussing the “shadow of a trial” theory of plea
bargaining, in which bargains are struck in anticipation of likely
trial outcomes and are aected by procedural safeguards inherent
in trial practice).
m Bowers, “Punishing the Innocent,” 2008, 1139-1140 (discussing
process costs in low-stakes cases for prosecutors, defense
attorneys, and judges) and 1132-1134 (discussing defendants
process costs in low-stakes cases). Also see Milton Heumann, Plea
Bargaining: The Experiences of Prosecutors, Judges and Defense
Attorneys, (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1978), 103 (finding
that the “central concern [for prosecutors] with . . . nonserious
cases is to dispose of them quickly). Heumann also noted that
people “in relatively minor cases . . . simply want to ‘get it over
with.’” Ibid. at 69. Also see Alexandra Natapo, “Misdemeanors,”
Southern California Law Review 85, no. 5 (2012), 1313, 1343-1347
(discussing why people feel pressure to plead guilty in low stakes
misdemeanor cases), https://perma.cc/Q3SF-NV8V; Issa Kohler-
Hausmann, “Managerial Justice and Mass Misdemeanors,”
Stanford Law Review 66, no. 3 (2014), 611, 655 and 691; Weinstein,
Adjudication of Minor Oenses,” 2004, 1157, 1162-1163; and Feeley,
The Process is Punishment, 1979, 31-33.
n See note m. It can also include the stigma associated with having
been arrested and a multitude of process costs related to mounting
a defense—including attorney’s fees, the inconvenience and
procedural hassle of multiple court appearances, and pecuniary
loss due to missed work or unanticipated cost outlays such as
those associated with transport or standby childcare. Bowers
also notes that the “lead up to even a misdemeanor trial may take
weeks or months,” some or all of which may be spent in jail. See
Bowers, “Punishing the Innocent,” 2008, 1133. Also see Heumann,
Plea Bargaining, 1978, 70 (discussing how people in minor cases
compare the cost of pleading guilty with the costs of fighting their
case and decide more often than not that pleading guilty is less
costly).
o Feeley, The Process is Punishment, 1979, 13 (describing some of the
reasons why it may be “rational” for people to plead guilty in petty
oense cases). Also see Bowers, “Punishing the Innocent,” 2008,
1139-1140.
p See for example Bowers, “Legal Guilt, Normative Innocence,” 2010,
1655; and Kohler-Hausmann, “Managerial Justice,” (2014), 611.
q Kohler-Hausmann, “Managerial Justice,” 2014, 611, 643 and 656
(“As the courts shifted away from the adjudicative model toward
the managerial model, criminal justice actors increasingly used the
misdemeanor process to mark, classify, and supervise people, often
without securing a conviction or imposing a sentence”).
“Challenges in researching plea bargaining” p. 25
a Besiki L. Kutateladze, Victoria Z. Lawson, and Nancy R. Andiloro,
“Does Evidence Really Matter? An Exploratory Analysis of the Role
of Evidence in Plea Bargaining in Felony Drug Cases,” Law and
Human Behavior 39, no. 5 (2015), 431-442, 431.
b Kutateladze, Lawson, and Andiloro, “Does Evidence Really Matter?,”
2015, 432. See generally Bruce Frederick and Don Stemen, The
Anatomy of Discretion: An Analysis of Prosecutorial Decision
Making – Technical Report (New York: Vera Institute of Justice,
In the Shadows: A Review of the Research on Plea Bargaining 55
2012), https://perma.cc/E6PW-YCWR, in which the researchers
found several discrepancies between how prosecutors viewed their
work and statistical analysis of actual case outcomes.
c See for example Allison D. Redlich, Alicia Summers, and Steven
Hoover, “Self-Reported False Confessions and False Guilty Pleas
Among Oenders with Mental Illness,” Law and Human Behavior 34,
no. 1 (2010), 79-90, 79 and 89.
d See for example Kutateladze, Lawson, and Andiloro, “Does Evidence
Really Matter?” 2015, 434.
e See Besiki Kutateladze, “Tracing Charge Trajectories: A Study of
the Influence of Race in Charge Changes at Case Screening,
Arraignment, and Disposition,” Criminology 56, no. 1 (2018), 123-153.
f Kutateladze, “Tracing Charge Trajectories,” 2018, 127.
g See Andrew Chongseh Kim, “Underestimating the Trial Penalty: An
Empirical Analysis of the Federal Trial Penalty and Critique of the
Abrams Study,” Mississippi Law Journal 84, no. 5 (2015), 1195-1256.
h Kutateladze, Lawson, and Andiloro, “Does Evidence Really Matter?”
2015, 440.
i Sonja Starr, “Estimating Gender Disparities in Federal Criminal
Cases,” University of Michigan Law & Economics Working Papers 57
(2012), 2 and 3, https://perma.cc/N5FP-JZBC.
j Kutateladze, Lawson, and Andiloro, “Does Evidence Really Matter?”
2015, 431-432.
k Don Stemen and Gipsy Escobar, “Whither the Prosecutor?
Prosecutor and County Eects on Guilty Plea Outcomes in
Wisconsin,” Justice Quarterly 35, no. 7 (2018), 1166-1194, 1168; Anne
Morrison Piehl and Shawn D. Bushway, “Measuring and Explaining
Charge Bargaining,” Journal of Quantitative Criminology 23, no.
2 (2007), 105-125; and Kutateladze, Lawson, and Andiloro, “Does
Evidence Really Matter?” 2015, 440.
“Displaced sentencing discretion? The impact of
sentencing guidelines on charge bargaining practices”
p. 36
a Until the late 1970s, judges had unfettered discretion to fashion
sentences subject only to statutory maximums. As a result,
criminal sentences were not only inconsistent and unpredictable,
but also rife with unwarranted disparities between similarly
situated oenders. Academic commentators highlighted a “gross
disparity in sentencing, with dierent sentences imposed upon
similar oenders who have committed similar oenses by the
same judge on dierent days, dierent judges on dierent days,
dierent judges on the same day, and dierent judges in dierent
jurisdictions.” See Richard Singer, “In Favor of ‘Presumptive
Sentences’ Set by a Sentencing Commission,” Crime & Delinquency
24, no. 4 (1978), 401, 402. In her dissent in Blakely v. Washington,
U.S. Supreme Court Justice Sandra Day O’Connor noted that
unguided sentencing discretion “inevitably resulted in severe
disparities in sentences received and served by defendants
committing the same oense and having similar criminal histories.
Indeed, rather than reflect legally relevant criteria, these disparities
too often were correlated with constitutionally suspect variables
such as race.” 542 U.S. 296, 315 (2004) (O’Connor, J., dissenting).
Judge Marvin Frankel criticized this highly discretionary approach
to sentencing as lawless and unfair in his book Criminal Sentences:
Law Without Order (New York: Hill & Wang, 1973), 8. For additional
discussion on the rationale for sentencing guidelines, see Steven
Chanenson, “The Next Era of Sentencing Reform,” Emory Law
Journal 54, no. 1 (2005), 377-460, 396-397, https://perma.cc/
365T-XJEX. For the most recent count of jurisdictions with
sentencing guidelines, see “Jurisdictions” tab at “Sentencing
Guidelines Resource Center,” Robina Institute of Criminal Law and
Criminal Justice, University of Minnesota, https://perma.cc/
Y6G8-RMFF.
b For the definition of “presumptive” sentencing guidelines, see
Chanenson, “The Next Era of Sentencing Reform,” 2005, 384. Also
see Jon Wool and Don Stemen, Aggravated Sentencing: Blakely v.
Washington: Practical Implications for State Sentencing Systems
(New York: Vera Institute of Justice, 2004), 2, https://perma.cc/
YC6U-UQTM.
c For definitions of “voluntary” sentencing guidelines, see
Chanenson, “The Next Era of Sentencing Reform,” 2005, 384. Also
see Wool and Stemen, Aggravated Sentencing, 2004, 2.
d For a discussion about these dierences, see Richard Frase,
“Varying Binding Eects of Guidelines: The Mandatory-to-Advisory
Continuum,” Robina Institute of Criminal Law and Criminal Justice,
University of Minnesota, March 25, 2015, https://perma.cc/HD5X-
YHP2.
e See for example Candace McCoy, “Determinate Sentencing, Plea
Bargaining Bans, and Hydraulic Discretion in California,” Justice
System Journal 9, no. 3 (1984), 256-275. Also see David Boerner,
“Sentencing Guidelines and Prosecutorial Discretion,” Judicature
78, no. 4 (1995), 196, 198, https://perma.cc/CK22-8BMQ.
f Terance D. Miethe, “Charging and Plea Bargaining Practices
under Determinate Sentencing: An Investigation of the Hydraulic
Displacement of Discretion,” Journal of Criminal Law & Criminology
78, no. 1 (1987), 155, 165-171, https://perma.cc/S2QZ-K4LP. Miethe
also found little evidence that extralegal attributes became more
important predictors of dispositions (severity of initial charges,
charge dismissals, charge reductions, sentence negotiations, and
overall plea bargains) after the guidelines were implemented.
g Ibid., 175.
h Unlike other states with presumptive sentencing guidelines, Ohio
does not have a sentencing grid. Presumptions, required findings
or factors, or other guidance, including sentencing ranges, are
contained in the criminal code for each criminal oense. John
Wooldredge and Timothy Grin, “Displaced Discretion Under
Ohio Sentencing Guidelines,” Journal of Criminal Justice 33
(2005), 301-316. See also John Wooldredge, Fritz Rauschenberg,
56 Vera Institute of Justice
Timothy Grin, and Travis Pratt, Impact of Ohio’s Senate Bill 2
on Sentencing Disparities (Washington, DC: U.S. Department of
Justice, National Institute of Justice, 2002), 4, https://perma.cc/
TLG8-UHU4.
i Wooldredge and Grin, “Displaced Discretion, Ohio,” 2005, 301-
316. Similar to Miethe’s findings, the researchers also found that
any increase in levels of prosecutorial discretion that might have
occurred did not result in substantive extralegal disparities in case
dispositions.
j Wooldredge and Grin, “Displaced Discretion, Ohio,” 2005, 301,
310.
k Washington, DC, implemented voluntary sentencing guidelines
in 2006 after a pilot period that started in 2004. Previously, the
district operated under an indeterminate sentencing structure
until 1999 and then a determinate system after discretionary
parole was abolished in 2000. See District of Columbia Sentencing
Commission, An Evaluation of the DC Voluntary Sentencing
Guidelines (Washington, DC: District of Columbia Sentencing
Commission, 2017), 5-6, https://perma.cc/G52E-S8FX.
l Stephen E. Vance and J. C. Oleson, “Displaced Discretion:
The Eects of Sentencing Guidelines on Prosecutors’ Charge
Bargaining in the District of Columbia Superior Court,” Criminal
Justice Policy Review 25, no. 3 (2014), 347-377.
m Stephen E. Vance, Kerry M. Richmond, and J. C. Oleson, “Weighing
the Value of the Bargain: Prosecutorial Discretion After Sentencing
Guidelines,” Criminal Justice Policy Review 30, no. 7 (2019).
n Vance and Oleson, “Displaced Discretion, DC,” 2014, 347, 368; and
Vance, Richmond and Oleson, “Weighing the Value of the Bargain,”
2019.
o Wooldredge and Grin, “Displaced Discretion, Ohio,” 2005, 301,
304; Vance and Oleson, “Displaced Discretion: DC,” 2014, 347, 353;
Vance, Richmond and Oleson, “Weighing the Value of the Bargain,”
2019; and Miethe, “Charging and Plea Bargaining Practices under
Determinate Sentencing,” 1987, 155, 161.
p Anne Morrison Piehl and Shawn D. Bushway, “Measuring
and Explaining Charge Bargaining,” Journal of Quantitative
Criminology 23, no. 2 (2007), 105, 118-119.
q Piehl and Bushway, “Measuring and Explaining Charge
Bargaining,” 2007, 105 and 117.
In the Shadows: A Review of the Research on Plea Bargaining 57
Endnotes
1 For a description of the typical adversarial common law trial,
see for example, Gerard E. Lynch, “Our Administrative System of
Criminal Justice,” Fordham Law Review 66, no. 6 (1998), 2117, 2118-
2119, https://perma.cc/YKC7-8TW3. For a discussion of how this
trial-centric vision of criminal justice is embedded in the popular
imagination and, in particular, cultural depictions of the system,
see Stephanos Bibas, The Machinery of Criminal Justice (New York:
Oxford University Press, 2012), xv-xvi.
2 For a discussion of how trials create public accountability for
both the accused and the government through the public airing
of charges and evidence, see Mary Jo White, “The Importance
of Trials to the Law and Public Accountability,” speech, United
States Securities and Exchange Commission, 5th Annual Judge A.
Flannery Lecture, November 14, 2013, https://perma.cc/YH5T-JLMU.
3 In 2018, only 2 percent of criminal cases resolved in federal court
went to trial. United States Courts, “U.S. District Courts—Judicial
Business 2018,” https://perma.cc/B33N-8DWE. The percentages
for most state courts were comparable. See Nicole Waters, Kathryn
Genthon, Sarah Gibson, and Diane Robinson, “Court Statistics
Project DataViewer,” database (Washington, DC: Court Statistics
Project, 2019), http://popup.ncsc.org/CSP/CSP_Intro.aspx.
4 For example, in large urban courts in 2009, the most recent year for
which data was gathered, 66 percent of people arrested on felony
charges were convicted—54 percent of a felony and the remaining
12 percent of a misdemeanor; 97 percent of these convictions
were the result of a plea. Brian A. Reaves, Felony Defendants in
Large Urban Counties, 2009 - Statistical Tables (Washington, DC:
Bureau of Justice Statistics (BJS), 2013), 22, 24, Table 21, https://
perma.cc/8PNS-WDA6. Of the 79,704 people whose criminal cases
were terminated in federal court in 2018, 73,109 were convicted.
71,550 (98 percent) of them pleaded guilty. United States Courts,
“U.S. District Courts—Judicial Business 2018.” Also see Bibas, The
Machinery of Criminal Justice, 2012.
5 For the proportion of guilty pleas that are a result of plea
bargaining, see Lindsey Devers, Plea and Charge Bargaining:
Research Summary (Washington, DC: Bureau of Justice Assistance,
2011), 1, https://perma.cc/TQR8-79JU. For the definition of plea
bargaining see, Black’s Law Dictionary 1270 (11th ed., 2014).
6 Joseph A. Colquitt, “Ad Hoc Plea Bargaining,” Tulane Law Review
75, no. 3 (2001), 695, 696.
7 Lafler v. Cooper, 566 U.S. 156, 170 (2012), https://perma.cc/
WQH6-TETJ.
8 Wesley MacNeil Oliver, “Toward a Common Law of Plea
Bargaining,” Kentucky Law Review 102, no. 1 (2013), 1, 31-37, https://
perma.cc/TE94-LAJ5 (discussing the near limitless discretion
prosecutors possess when making charging and plea bargaining
decisions). For definitions, see Marc Miller, Ronald Wright et al.,
Criminal Procedures, Prosecutions, and Adjudications: Cases,
Statutes and Executive Materials, 6th ed. (New York: Wolters Kluwer,
2019), 309 and 363.
9 Gerard E. Lynch, “Screening Versus Plea Bargaining: Exactly What
Are We Trading O?,” Stanford Law Review 55 (2003) 1399, 1403
(arguing that most plea negotiations are discussions of the merits
of the case in which defense attorneys point out legal, evidentiary,
or practical weaknesses in the prosecutor’s case, or mitigating
circumstances that merit mercy), https://perma.cc/6FUH-VZVV. Also
see Cynthia Alkon, “What’s Law Got to Do with It? Plea Bargaining
Reform after Lafler and Frye,” Arbitration Law Review 7, no. 1 (2015),
10, https://perma.cc/X4WG-DW8L.
10 See for example John Lande, “A Framework for Advancing
Negotiation Theory: Implications from a Study of How Lawyers
Reach Agreement in Pretrial Litigation,” Cardozo Journal of Conflict
Resolution 16, no. 1 (2014) 1, 15-16 and footnote 47, https://perma.cc/
2PNY-ZWMZ.
11 Alkon, “What’s Law Got to Do with It?”, 2015, 10.
12 For “blind plea,” see LegalMatch, “Blind plea,” https://perma.
cc/6CG9-MSTQ. For “pleading the sheet,” see Laura Berend,
“Less Reliable Preliminary Hearings and Plea Bargains in Criminal
Cases in California: Discovery Before and After Proposition 115,”
American University Law Review 48, no. 2 (1998), 465, notes 211
and 227, https://perma.cc/SN9E-CH76. For a discussion on the
varied meanings of an “open plea,” see Harper v. State, 567 S.W.3d
450, 454-455 (Tex. App. 2019), (discussing how one of the two
meanings refers to “pleas where no plea bargaining of any kind has
occurred”), https://perma.cc/6LFD-UJH8.
13 Stephanos Bibas, “Transparency and Participation in Criminal
Procedure,” New York University Law Review 81, no. 3 (2006),
911, 923 (“Plea bargaining usually occurs in conference rooms,
courtroom hallways, or on private telephone calls instead of open
court”), https://perma.cc/2LSS-5ASD. Also see Stephanos Bibas,
“Plea Bargaining Outside the Shadow of Trial,” Harvard Law Review
117, no. 8 (2004), 2463, 2547 (“plea bargaining hides within a low-
visibility process”), https://perma.cc/GA76-3SQX; Robert E. Scott
and William J. Stuntz, “Plea Bargaining as Contract,” Yale Law
Journal 101, no. 8 (1992), 1909, 1911-1912, (“Most cases are disposed
of by means that seem scandalously casual: a quick conversation
in a prosecutor’s oce or a courthouse hallway between attorneys
familiar with only the basics of the case, with no witnesses
present”); and Daniel S. McConkie, “Judges As Framers of Plea
Bargaining,” Stanford Law and Policy Review 26, no. 1 (2015), 61, 63,
(“Plea bargaining happens with little judicial involvement—between
prosecutors and defense attorneys, behind closed doors and with
practically no public oversight”), https://perma.cc/QV6X-EQTS. See
also Alkon, “What’s Law Got to Do with It?,” 2015, 1, 11 (discussing
a 1984 court ethnography by Douglas Maynard, which found that
plea negotiations happened in “spurts and starts” in between other
conversations).
14 Scott and Stuntz, “Plea Bargaining as Contract,” 1992, 1909,
1912 (discussing critiques of the illicit and hidden nature of plea
bargaining: “critics emphasize the unfairness (and inaccuracy)
58 Vera Institute of Justice
of determining defendants’ fate without full investigation, without
testimony and evidence and impartial factfinding”).
15 H. Mitchell Caldwell, “Coercive Plea Bargaining: The Unrecognized
Scourge of the Justice System,” Catholic University Law Review 61,
no. 1 (2011), 63, 82-83, https://perma.cc/XU2L-8L38.
16 Oliver, “Toward A Common Law,” 2013, 1.
17 See for example Carlos Berdejó, “Criminalizing Race: Racial
Disparities in Plea-Bargaining,” Boston College Law Review 59, no. 4
(2018), 1187, https://perma.cc/E6D8-ZFFJ.
18 Jacob Kang-Brown and Ram Subramanian, Out of Sight: The
Growth of Jails in Rural America (New York: Vera Institute of Justice,
2017), 9-10, https://perma.cc/34ME-RRRT.
19 For a review of these studies and their findings, see Léon Digard
and Elizabeth Swavola, Justice Denied: The Harmful and Lasting
Eects of Pretrial Detention (New York: Vera Institute of Justice,
2019), 2-6, https://perma.cc/N42X-TMLK.
20 Some researchers have argued that pretrial detention weakens a
person’s bargaining power during the plea negotiation stage and
highlight the lack of work/income, family contact, and access
to social support during incarceration, plus the likelihood of
immediate release using time served for sentences given for lower-
level charges. See for example Will Dobbie, Jacob Goldin, and
Crystal S. Yang, “The Eects of Pretrial Detention on Conviction,
Future Crime, and Employment: Evidence from Randomly Assigned
Judges.” American Economic Review 108, no. 2 (2018), 201-240,
https://perma.cc/Y2B7-BSN8; and Emily Leslie and Nolan G. Pope,
“The Unintended Impact of Pretrial Detention on Case Outcomes:
Evidence from New York City Arraignments,” Journal of Law and
Economics 60, no. 3 (2017), 529-557, https://perma.cc/C8NR-GGB8.
21 Meghan Sacks and Alissa R. Ackerman, “Pretrial Detention and
Guilty Pleas: If They Cannot Aord Bail, They Must Be Guilty,”
Criminal Justice Studies 25, no. 3, (2012), 265, 272-273.
22 Ellen A. Donnelly and John M. MacDonald, “The Downstream
Eects of Bail and Pretrial Detention on Racial Disparities in
Incarceration,” Journal of Criminal Law & Criminology 108, no. 4
(2018), 775, 805, https://perma.cc/M3MF-RTKV.
23 For previous research, see for example Celesta A. Albonetti, “Race
and the Probability of Pleading Guilty,” Journal of Quantitative
Criminology 6, no. 3 (1990), 315-334.
24 Albonetti, “Race and the Probability of Pleading Guilty,” (1990),
315, 329-332.
25 For a definition of a natural experiment, see Thad Dunning,
“Improving Causal Inference Strengths and Limitations of Natural
Experiments,” Political Research Quarterly 61, no. 2 (2008), 282-93,
282-83. Note that although two of the studies (Leslie and Pope,
“Unintended Impact,” (2017); and Dobbie, Goldin, and Yang, “The
Eects of Pre-Trial Detention,” 2018) demonstrated that a quasi-
experimental design can improve estimates based on regression
analysis, the other two studies (Paul Heaton, Sandra G. Mayson,
and Megan Stevenson, “The Downstream Consequences of
Misdemeanor Pretrial Detention,” Stanford Law Review 69, no.
3 (2017), https://perma.cc/Y4P4-GQUY; and Megan Stevenson,
“Distortion of Justice: How the Inability to Pay Bail Aects Case
Outcomes,” working paper (Philadelphia, PA: University of
Pennsylvania Law School, November 8, 2016), https://perma.cc/
W4EC-R9MW) found that the dierences between the findings of
their natural experiments and regression analyses were either not
statistically significant or did not vary substantially. This suggests
that regression analyses can produce reasonable causal estimates
that link pretrial detention to a person’s decision to plead guilty
when sucient controls are deployed.
26 The major assumption on which these studies are based is that the
likelihood of being detained pretrial is exogenously aected by the
magistrate who presides over the bail hearing.
27 Stevenson, “Distortion of Justice,” 2016, 3 and 17.
28 Leslie and Pope, “Unintended Impact,” 2017, 529, 543 and 547. The
estimated eect of detention on pleading guilty is only about 25
percent smaller than the eect on conviction, which suggests that
detention primarily aects conviction by inducing some people
who would not have pled guilty if released to plead guilty after they
are detained.
29 Leslie and Pope, “Unintended Impact,” 2017, 529, 548.
30 Leslie and Pope, “Unintended Impact,” 2017, 529, 552.
31 Heaton, Mayson, and Stevenson, “Downstream Consequences,”
2017, 711, 771 and 776.
32 Dobbie, Goldin, and Yang, “The Eects of Pre-Trial Detention,” 2018,
201, 225.
33 Will Dobbie, Jacob Goldin and Crystal Yang, “The Eects of Pre-
Trial Detention on Conviction, Future Crime, and Employment:
Evidence from Randomly Assigned Judges,” Working Paper No.
22511 (Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research,
2016), 23-24, https://perma.cc/XN26-HFBJ.
34 Dobbie, Goldin, and Yang, “The Eects of Pre-Trial Detention,” 2018,
201, 225 and 234.
35 Heaton, Mayson, and Stevenson, “Downstream Consequences,”
2017, 711-794, 747. Also see Dobbie, Goldin, and Yang, “The Eects of
Pre-Trial Detention,” Working Paper No. 22511, 2016, 25-26.
36 Heaton, Mayson, and Stevenson, “Downstream Consequences,”
2017, 711-794, 749.
37 Stevenson, “Distortion of Justice,” 2016, 1, 21-22 and Table 7 on p.
35.
38 Dobbie, Goldin, and Yang, “The Eects of Pre-Trial Detention,”
Working Paper No. 22511, 2016, 23-24.
39 Dobbie, Goldin, and Yang, “The Eects of Pre-Trial Detention,” 2018,
201, 230.
In the Shadows: A Review of the Research on Plea Bargaining 59
40 Leslie and Pope, “Unintended Impact,” 2017, 529, 542, 553-
54. However, the researchers found that, unlike the felony
subsample, first-time oender status is not a source of substantial
heterogeneity. If anything, people without a criminal history were
less aected by pretrial detention. Ibid., 548, Table 10.
41 Ibid.
42 Susan Ehrhard, “Plea Bargaining and the Death Penalty: An
Exploratory Study,” Justice System Journal 29, no. 3 (2008), 313-
325, 314.
43 Ehrhard, “Plea Bargaining and the Death Penalty,” 2008, 313-325,
314 and 316.
44 Ilyana Kuziemko, “Does the Threat of the Death Penalty Aect
Plea Bargaining in Murder Cases? Evidence From New York’s
1995 Reinstatement of Capital Punishment,” American Law and
Economics Review 8, no. 1 (2006), 116-142.
45 Kuziemko, “Does the Threat of the Death Penalty Aect Plea
Bargaining,” 2006, 116-142, 126. Confidence in the assumption that
this change was the result, at least in part, of changes in the death
penalty law is increased by the finding that plea behavior did not
change for people charged with robbery, rape, or burglary over the
same time period.
46 Kuziemko, “Does the Threat of the Death Penalty Aect Plea
Bargaining,” 2006, 128.
47 Sherod Thaxton, “Leveraging Death,” The Journal of Criminal Law
and Criminology 103, no. 2 (2013), 475-552.
48 Thaxton, “Leveraging Death,” 2013, 525.
49 Ibid., 537.
50 Kent S. Scheidegger, “The Death Penalty and Plea Bargaining to
Life Sentences,” Working Paper 09-01 (Sacramento, CA: Criminal
Justice Legal Foundation, 2009), 10, https://perma.cc/RNM8-TAFG.
51 Scheidegger, “Plea Bargaining to Life Sentences,” 2009, 13.
52 Dean J. Champion, “Private Counsels and Public Defenders: A Look
at Weak Cases, Prior Records, and Leniency in Plea Bargaining,”
Journal of Criminal Justice 17, no. 4 (1989), 256, 257.
53 Allison D. Redlich, Shawn D. Bushway, and Robert J. Norris,
“Plea Decision-Making by Attorneys and Judges,” Journal of
Experimental Criminology 12, no. 4 (2016), 537-561, 548-549.
54 Redlich, Bushway, and Norris, “Plea Decision-Making by Attorneys
and Judges,” 2016, 552.
55 Ibid., 556 and 557.
56 Bruce Frederick and Don Stemen, The Anatomy of Discretion: An
Analysis of Prosecutorial Decision Making – Technical Report
(New York: The Vera Institute of Justice, 2012), https://perma.cc/
H6A2-ET78.
57 Ibid., 202-219.
58 Ibid., 245.
59 Besiki L. Kutateladze, Victoria Z. Lawson, and Nancy R. Andiloro,
“Does Evidence Really Matter? An Exploratory Analysis of the Role
of Evidence in Plea Bargaining in Felony Drug Cases,” Law and
Human Behavior 39, no. 5 (2015), 431-442, 440. This study was not
able to quantify the strength of the evidence, merely the existence
of evidence.
60 Ibid., 436.
61 Ibid., 441.
62 Allison D. Redlich, Shi Yan, Robert J. Norris, and Shawn D. Bushway,
“The Influence of Confessions on Guilty Pleas and Plea Discounts,”
Psychology, Public Policy, and Law 24, no. 2 (2018), 147-157, 148.
63 Ibid., 147-157, 153. People who maintained their innocence to the
police but pled guilty received the largest sentence discounts. Ibid.,
155. It is possible that a proportion of these people were factually
innocent or otherwise had weak cases against them, incentivizing
prosecutors to oer greater discounts in order to secure a
conviction. People who provided only partial confessions received
worse plea deals than those who fully confessed. Ibid., 152. The
researchers hypothesized that such people were seen as being less
remorseful than people who gave full confessions and received less
leniency as a result. Ibid., 155.
64 Ibid., 153. For earlier studies, see Redlich, Bushway, and Norris,
“Plea Decision-Making by Attorneys and Judges,” 2016.
65 Redlich, Yan, Norris, and Bushway, “Confessions,” 2018, 155.
66 Kutateladze, Lawson, and Andiloro, “Does Evidence Really Matter?”
2015, 431.
67 Ibid., 439.
68 Frederick and Stemen, The Anatomy of Discretion, 2012, 200-201.
69 See Albonetti, “Race and the Probability of Pleading Guilty,” (1990),
315-334, 317; and Jon’a Meyer and Tara Gray, “Drunk Drivers in
the Courts: Legal and Extra-Legal Factors Aecting Pleas and
Sentences,” Journal of Criminal Justice 25, no. 2 (1997), 157-160.
70 Meyer and Gray, “Drunk Drivers in the Courts,” (1997), 157.
71 Ibid., 156, 161. The researchers caution, however, that the sentencing
guidelines for the specific oense studied may be highly
influential in this decision-making process and may, therefore, limit
generalizability to other oenses or other jurisdictions.
72 Albonetti, “Race and the Probability of Pleading Guilty,” 1990, 323.
73 Lauren O’Neill Shermer and Brian D. Johnson “Criminal
Prosecutions: Examining Prosecutorial Discretion and Charge
Reductions in U.S. Federal District Courts,” Justice Quarterly 27, no.
3 (2010), 20, https://perma.cc/T6B9-VUFA. The sample consisted
of 45,678 sentencing events and included felonies and class A
misdemeanors.
60 Vera Institute of Justice
74 Shermer and Johnson, “Prosecutorial Discretion and Charge
Reductions,” 2010, 27.
75 For an example of a jurisdiction that incorporates criminal history
into its plea bargaining guidelines, see Besiki L. Kutateladze, Nancy
R. Andiloro, Brian D. Johnson, and Cassia C. Spohn, “Cumulative
Disadvantage: Examining Racial and Ethnic Disparity in Prosecution
and Sentencing,” Criminology 52, no. 3 (2014), 514-551, 523. For
information about jurisdictions that incorporate criminal history
into sentencing guidelines, see generally Neal B. Kauder and Brian
J. Ostram, State Sentencing Guidelines: Profiles and Continuum
(Washington, DC: National Center for State Courts, 2008), https://
perma.cc/HG2G-GNCT.
76 Jeery T. Ulmer, James Eisenstein, and Brian D. Johnson, “Trial
Penalties in Federal Sentencing: Extra-Guidelines Factors and
District Variation,” Justice Quarterly 27, no. 4 (2009), 1-33, 19,
https://perma.cc/LMR4-2QCG; and Jeery T. Ulmer and Mindy S.
Bradley-Engen, “Variation in Trial Penalties Among Serious Violent
Oenses,” Criminology 44, no. 3 (2006), 631-670, 653-654.
77 Ulmer and Bradley-Engen, “Variation in Trial Penalties,” 2006, 661;
and Ulmer, Eisenstein, and Johnson, “Trial Penalties in Federal
Sentencing,” 2010, 584-585.
78 Shermer and Johnson, “Prosecutorial Discretion and Charge
Reductions,” 2010, 20.
79 Kutateladze, Andiloro, Johnson, and Spohn, “Cumulative
Disadvantage,” 2014, 532.
80 Frederick and Stemen, The Anatomy of Discretion, 2012, 217.
81 Ibid., 204 and 209.
82 Kutateladze, Andiloro, Johnson, and Spohn, “Cumulative
Disadvantage,” (2014), 538.
83 Ibid.
84 Besiki Kutateladze, Nancy R. Andiloro, and Brian D. Johnson,
“Opening Pandora’s Box: How Does Defendant Race Influence Plea
Bargaining?” Justice Quarterly 33, no. 3 (2016); and Kutateladze,
Andiloro, Johnson, and Spohn, “Cumulative Disadvantage,” 2014.
85 Digard and Swavola, Justice Denied, 2019, 7.
86 Donnelly and MacDonald, “The Downstream Eects of Bail,” 2018;
and Kutateladze, Andiloro, Johnson, and Spohn, “Cumulative
Disadvantage,” 2014.
87 Carlos Berdejó, “Criminalizing Race,” 2018, 1187-1249.
88 Ibid., 1222.
89 Ibid., 1226-1227.
90 Ibid., 1238.
91 Kutateladze, Andiloro, and Johnson, “Pandora’s Box,” 2016;
and Kutateladze, Andiloro, Johnson, and Spohn, “Cumulative
Disadvantage,” 2014.
92 Kutateladze, Andiloro, Johnson, and Spohn, “Cumulative
Disadvantage,” 2014. This study found harsher treatment for Black
people at all stages of prosecution, with one exception—they were
significantly more likely than white people to have their cases
dismissed. The authors speculate that this could be the result of a
greater proportion of inappropriate arrests for Black people, which
then lack sucient evidentiary strength to continue. Alternatively,
the authors also pose the possibility that victims of or witnesses
to crimes in which the arrests were made may be less willing to
cooperate with an investigation due to mistrust of the police. See
ibid., 538, for more information.
93 Jeremy D. Ball, “Is it a Prosecutor’s World? Determinants of Count
Bargaining Decisions,” Journal of Contemporary Criminal Justice
22, no. 3 (2006), 241-260, 256.
94 Shermer and Johnson, “Prosecutorial Discretion and Charge
Reductions,” 2010, 394-430. Although this study did not find proof
of racial bias across all cases in the aggregate, it did uncover
systematically poorer treatment of Black and Latinx people in
plea bargaining for specific oenses. A later study examined a
variety of metrics of treatment by prosecutors, including whether
prosecutors recommended a custodial sentence and reductions in
the seriousness of the top charge or the sentence length this charge
could lead to. The researchers found evidence of harsher treatment
towards Black and Latinx people for some oense types using
some metrics of treatment, but not all. Frederick and Stemen, The
Anatomy of Discretion, 2012.
95 Shermer and Johnson, “Criminal Prosecutions,” 2010, 415.
96 Berdejó, “Criminalizing Race,” 2018, 1187; and Kutateladze, Lawson,
and Andiloro, “Does Evidence Really Matter?” 2015, 439.
97 Danielle Rousseau and Gerald Pezzullo, “Race and Context in
the Criminal Labeling of Drunk Driving Oenders: A Multilevel
Examination of Extralegal Variables on Discretionary Plea
Decisions,” Criminal Justice Policy Review 25, no. 6 (2014), https://
perma.cc/H4DK-RX2F.
98 Rousseau and Pezzullo, “Drunk Driving Oenders,” 2014, 696.
99 Besiki Kutateladze, “Tracing Charge Trajectories: A Study of
the Influence of Race in Charge Changes at Case Screening,
Arraignment, and Disposition,” Criminology 56, no. 1 (2018), 123-
153.
100 Kutateladze, “Tracing Charge Trajectories,” 2018, 127 and 145-146.
Furthermore, Black and Latinx people were more likely than white
people to see their charges increased at screening—perhaps, the
researchers theorize, due to a greater perceived threat to public
safety posed by them or as a tactical move by prosecutors to
create a charging baseline that will make someone more likely to
accept a plea deal.
101 Kutateladze, “Tracing Charge Trajectories,” 2018, 145.
In the Shadows: A Review of the Research on Plea Bargaining 61
102 Sonja Starr, “Estimating Gender Disparities in Federal Criminal
Cases,” University of Michigan Law & Economics Working Papers 57
(2012), 3, https://perma.cc/25J2-CEMQ.
103 See for example Donna Bishop and Charles Frazier, “The Eects
of Gender on Charge Reduction,” Sociological Quarterly 25, no. 3
(1984), 385-396. A second wave of data collection was needed to
secure a large enough sample of cases with female defendants.
104 Celesta A. Albonetti, “Charge Reduction: An Analysis of
Prosecutorial Discretion in Burglary and Robbery Cases,” Journal
of Quantitative Criminology 8 (1992), 317-333; Bishop and Frazier,
“The Eects of Gender on Charge Reduction,” 1984; Ball, “Is it a
Prosecutor’s World?” 2006; and Jerey W. Spears and Cassia C.
Spohn, “The Eect of Evidence Factors and Victim Characteristics
on Prosecutors’ Charging Decisions in Sexual Assault Cases,”
Justice Quarterly 14, no. 3 (1997), 501-524. Spears and Spohn
used data from the 1970s collected in Michigan and focused on
violent felonies. Although initial analysis showed women to be
more likely to receive a reduction in their charges, this was not
found to be significant when modeled with other variables (see
pages 42 and 43). See also Erika Davis Frenzel and Jeremy D. Ball,
“Eects of Individual Characteristics on Plea Negotiations Under
Sentencing Guidelines,” Journal of Ethnicity in Criminal Justice 5,
no. 4 (2008), 59-82. This sample of 1998 data from Pennsylvania
showed that men were more likely to go to trial than women, but
the researchers did not find a significant relationship between
people’s demographic characteristics and prosecutors’ plea
bargaining decisions. Ibid., 75. However, the researchers noted that
the analysis was unable to distinguish “true” plea bargains (which
can be expected to have a measurable eect on sentencing) and
“symbolic” plea bargains (in which charges are reduced in ways
that ultimately provide little meaningful benefit to the accused).
The researchers hypothesized that disentangling these two could
reveal disparities in the treatment of dierent groups of people.
Ibid., 77.
105 See Carlos Berdejó, “Gender Disparities in Plea Bargaining,”
Indiana Law Journal 94, no. 4 (2019), https://perma.cc/7N7C-RA93;
Shermer and Johnson, “Criminal Prosecutions,” 2010; Frederick and
Stemen, The Anatomy of Discretion, 2012; and Starr, “Estimating
Gender Disparities,” 2012. Whether this general shift is the result
of changes in court practice or deepening nuance in how plea
bargaining is understood and researched is unclear.
106 Berdejó, “Gender Disparities,” 2019, 1272, 1279, and 1283.
107 Ibid., 1260-61.
108 Ibid., 1265-66.
109 Shermer and Johnson, “Criminal Prosecutions,” 2010, 20.
110 See Starr, “Estimating Gender Disparities,” 2012, 12-16; and Berdejó,
“Gender Disparities,” 2019, 1259-1260.
111 Starr, “Estimating Gender Disparities,” 2012, 17.
112 Rebecca K. Helm, Valerie F. Reyna, Allison A. Franz, and Rachel
Z. Novick, “Too Young to Plead? Risk, Rationality, and Plea
Bargaining’s Innocence Problem in Adolescents,” Psychology,
Public Policy, and Law 24, no. 2 (2018), 180-191, 187. In another study,
interviews conducted with 40 juveniles who had either accepted
a guilty plea or were currently considering a plea oer shed more
light on this issue. They had an incomplete understanding of what
a plea bargain is. Although participants understood that accepting
a plea was considered an admission of guilt and could lead to a
reduction in charges or lesser punishment, they frequently did
not understand that they were also waiving their right to trial and
appeal. In explaining the factors that influenced their decisions
to plead guilty or not, the majority of participants mentioned
short-term consequences (wanting to go home, being tired of the
legal process, etc.) and only 17 percent mentioned any long-term
consequences (such as the impact of having a criminal history).
Tarika Daftary-Kapur and Tina M. Zottoli, “A First Look at the Plea
Deal Experiences of Juveniles Tried in Adult Court,” International
Journal of Forensic Mental Health 13, no. 4 (2014), 323-336, 328-33.
113 Ibid., 333.
114 Erika N. Fountain and Jennifer L. Woolard, “How Defense Attorneys
Consult with Juvenile Clients About Plea Bargains,” Psychology,
Public Policy, and Law 24, no. 2 (2018), 192-203.
115 Fountain and Woolard, “Consult with Juvenile Clients,” 2018, 197.
116 Tina M. Zottoli, Tarika Daftary-Kapur, Georgia M. Winters, and
Conor Hogan, “Plea Discounts, Time Pressures, and False-Guilty
Pleas in Youth and Adults Who Pleaded Guilty to Felonies in New
York City,” Psychology, Public Policy, and Law 22, no. 3 (2016), 250-
259, 254-255.
117 Frederick and Stemen, The Anatomy of Discretion, 2012.
118 Ibid., 208, 211, 214, and 217.
119 See Rachel Barkow, “Institutional Design and the Policing of
Prosecutors: Lessons from Administrative Law,” Stanford Law Review
61, no. 4 (2010), 869-922, https://perma.cc/MPZ3-TSWF; and Rachel
Barkow, “Separation of Powers and the Criminal Law,” Stanford
Law Review 58, no.4 (2006), 989-1054, 1024-1028, https://perma.cc/
CB5W-DZAP. Also see Erik Luna and Marianne Wade, “Prosecutors
as Judges,” Washington and Lee Law Review 67, no. 4 (2010), 1413,
1414-1418, https://perma.cc/G67T-WAPN.
120 For a discussion of the concept that plea bargaining is “lawless,”
see Andrew Crespo, “The Hidden Law of Plea Bargaining,”
Columbia Law Review 118, no. 5 (2018), 1303, 1304-1307, https://
perma.cc/NDV3-4XLB. For a discussion of the eects of this decision
making, see for example Berdejó, “Gender Disparities,” 2019; and
Berdejó, “Criminalizing Race,” 2018.
121 See for example William Stuntz, “Plea Bargaining and Criminal
Law’s Disappearing Shadow,” Harvard Law Review 117, no. 8 (2004)
2548-2569, 2548; Bibas, “Plea Bargaining Outside the Shadow of
Trial,” (2004), 2463-2547; and Tracey Meares, “Rewards for Good
62 Vera Institute of Justice
Behavior: Influencing Prosecutorial Discretion and Conduct with
Financial Incentives,” Fordham Law Review 64, no. 3 (1995), 851,
862-865, https://perma.cc/3RUC-J5VV.
122 Ronald Wright and Rodney Engen, “The Eects of Depth and
Distance in a Criminal Code on Charging, Sentencing and
Prosecutor Power,” North Carolina Law Review 84, no. 6 (2006),
1935-1982, https://perma.cc/4AR3-QP3H; and Ronald Wright and
Rodney Engen, “Charge Movement and Theories of Prosecutors,”
Marquette Law Review 91, no. 1 (2007), 9-38, https://perma.cc/
EB55-USHY.
123 Wright and Engen, “The Eects of Depth and Distance,” 2006, 1940.
124 Ibid.
125 Ibid., 1958.
126 Ibid.,1959-1967.
127 Ibid., 1964 and 1967.
128 Ibid., 1967.
129 Wright and Engen, “Charge Movement and Theories of
Prosecutors,” 2007, 25-26, https://perma.cc/EB55-USHY.
130 Ibid., 26-27.
131 Kyle Graham, “Crimes, Widgets, and Plea Bargaining: An Analysis
of Charge Content, Pleas, and Trials,” California Law Review 100,
no. 6 (2012), 1573, 1577-1578, https://perma.cc/3XNL-HK44.
132 Ibid., 1597 and 1602-3.
133 Ibid.
134 Ibid. The study also found that prosecutors use some of these
crimes, such as misprision of a felony, as a “pleading crime”—a
crime that is rarely the basis for an initial arrest or the most serious
charge in charging documents filed with the court (if it appears at
all), but is used as the basis of a plea deal once other charges are
dropped. Ibid., 1605.
135 Ibid., 1608 and 1618-1619.
136 Ibid., 1605-1613.
137 Ibid., 1582-1583 and 1613. A “sentencing cli” occurs when the
shortest sentence for one charge might still be significantly longer
than the longest sentence for the next most serious charge or might
mandate incarceration when the other charge does not.
138 This was also the case with civil rights deprivation cases. Ibid., 1616-
1617.
139 For tax evasion, see ibid., 1625. For sexual oenses, see ibid., 1596.
140 See for example Jerold H. Israel “Excessive Criminal Justice
Caseloads: Challenging the Conventional Wisdom,” Florida Law
Review 48, no. 5 (1996), 761-779, 764, https://perma.cc/5BQJ-V2JT.
141 In fact, the system is so dependent on plea bargaining over trial
that recently, scholars have suggested that “crashing the courts”—
insisting on right to trial for every accused person—would be
an eective means of protesting incarceration by bringing the
entire mechanism of the justice system to a halt. See for example
Michelle Alexander, “Go to Trial: Crash the Justice System,” New
York Times, March 10, 2012, https://www.nytimes.com/2012/03/11/
opinion/sunday/go-to-trial-crash-the-justice-system.html. See also
John H. Blume and Rebecca K. Helm, “The Unexonerated: Factually
Innocent Defendants Who Plead Guilty” Cornell Law Review 100,
no. 1 (2014), 157-191, (“More than forty years ago, then Chief Justice
Warren Burger stated that ‘a reduction from 90% to 80% in guilty
pleas requires the assignment of twice the judicial manpower and
facilities’ and, ‘a reduction to 70% trebles the demand.’” (citing
Warren E. Burger, “The State of the Judiciary,” A.B.A. Journal 56
(1970) 929, 931)), https://perma.cc/9AD9-TGX9.
142 Israel, “Excessive Criminal Justice Caseloads,” 1996, 767.
143 See generally Heumann, The Experiences of Prosecutors, Judges,
and Defense Attorneys, 1978.
144 Peter F. Nardulli, “The Caseload Controversy and the Study of
Criminal Courts,” Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology 70,
no. 1 (1979), 89-101, 95, https://perma.cc/9JCL-DKZ4. In this study
caseloads were calculated both per judge and per court, though
the caseloads of individual prosecutors were not analyzed.
145 Ulmer and Bradley-Engen, “Variation in Trial Penalties,” 2006, 650;
and Ulmer, Eisenstein, and Johnson, “Trial Penalties in Federal
Sentencing,” 2010, 579.
146 Ulmer, Eisenstein, and Johnson, “Trial Penalties in Federal
Sentencing,” 2010, 588.
147 Kutateladze, Andiloro, and Johnson, “Pandora’s Box,” 2016; and
Kutateladze, Lawson, and Andiloro, “Does Evidence Really Matter?”
2015, 431-442.
148 Kutateladze, Andiloro, and Johnson, “Pandora’s Box,” 2016, 409;
and Kutateladze, Lawson, and Andiloro, “Does Evidence Really
Matter?” 2015, 436.
149 Kutateladze, Lawson, and Andiloro, “Does Evidence Really Matter?”
2015, 439.
150 Ibid., 440.
151 Kutateladze, Andiloro, and Johnson, “Pandora’s Box,” 2016, 418.
152 Don Stemen and Gipsy Escobar, “Whither the Prosecutor?
Prosecutor and County Eects on Guilty Plea Outcomes in
Wisconsin,” Justice Quarterly 35, no. 7 (2018), 1166-1194, 1175-1178.
153 Ibid., 1181.
154 Ibid., 1181 and 1182.
155 Ibid., 1182.
156 Ibid., 1184.
157 Ibid., 1181 and 1182.
In the Shadows: A Review of the Research on Plea Bargaining 63
158 Ibid., 1184 and 1185.
159 Ibid.
160 See for example Frederick and Stemen, The Anatomy of Discretion,
2012, 277-280; and Gary D. LaFree, “Adversarial and Nonadversarial
Justice: A Comparison of Guilty Pleas and Trials,” Criminology 23,
no. 2 (1985), 289-312, 306.
161 Frederick and Stemen, The Anatomy of Discretion, 2012, 277-280.
162 LaFree, “Adversarial and Nonadversarial Justice,” 1985, 306.
163 See Shi Yan and Shawn D. Bushway, “Plea Discounts or Trial
Penalties? Making Sense of the Trial-Plea Sentence Disparities,”
Justice Quarterly 35, no. 7 (2018), 1226-1249. Beyond the sentence
or charge reductions negotiated with prosecutors during plea
bargaining, dierences in outcomes may also result from sentence
increases at trial; this may happen if a judge responds negatively
to a defendant or a trial attracts significant attention from the
public. See Nancy J. King, David A. Soulé, Sara Steen, and Robert
R. Weidner, “When Process Aects Punishment: Dierences in
Sentences After Guilty Plea, Bench Trial, and Jury Trial in Five
Guidelines States,” Columbia Law Review 105 (2005), 959-1009,
964, https://perma.cc/K4CT-4FMR.
164 See for example King, Soulé, Steen, and Weidner, “When Process
Aects Punishment,” 2005, 959-1009.
165 For an example of a weak relationship between pleading guilty
and sentencing outcomes, see Celesta A. Albonetti, “Sentencing
Under the Federal Sentencing Guidelines: Eects of Defendant
Characteristics, Guilty Pleas, and Departures on Sentence
Outcomes for Drug Oenses, 1991-1992,” Law and Society Review
31, no. 4 (1997), 789-822. For a critique of the methodology used in
this and other studies, see Andrew Chongseh Kim, “Underestimating
the Trial Penalty: An Empirical Analysis of the Federal Trial Penalty
and Critique of the Abrams Study,” Mississippi Law Journal 84, no.
5 (2015), 1195-1256.
166 Jeery T. Ulmer and Mindy S. Bradley-Engen, “Variations in Trial
Penalties Among Serious Violent Oenses,” Criminology 44, no. 3
(2006), 631-670.
167 Ibid., 650.
168 Ibid., 652.
169 See King, Soulé, Steen, and Weidner, “When Process Aects
Punishment,” 2005, 959-1009.
170 See for example LaFree, “Adversarial and Nonadversarial Justice,”
1985, 289-312, 293-294. Although the United States is not the only
country to incorporate plea bargaining as part of its criminal legal
system, other countries such as Germany have established laws
limiting the bargaining power of prosecutors and incorporating
judicial oversight in both the bargaining and sentencing phase.
By contrast, few states mandate more than the de minimis judicial
inquiry into whether a plea is “knowing, voluntary, and intelligent.”
This leaves plea bargaining largely in the discretion of and
subject to the policies of individual prosecutors’ oces. See for
example Jenia Turner, “Judicial Participation in Plea Negotiations:
A Comparative View,” American Journal of Comparative Law 54
(2006), 501-570, https://perma.cc/JGU9-3A9W.
171 The five states were Kansas, Maryland, Minnesota, Pennsylvania,
and Washington. King, Soulé, Steen, and Weidner, “When Process
Aects Punishment,” 2005.
172 Ibid., 975.
173 Ibid., 973.
174 Ibid.
175 Ibid., 974.
176 Ibid., 975.
177 See Ulmer, Eisenstein, and Johnson, “Trial Penalties in Federal
Sentencing,” 2010, 565. The authors identified other sentencing
guidelines that may especially benefit people who plead guilty,
such as sentence reduction for people who render “substantial
assistance” to law enforcement. Conversely, people who “obstruct
justice” by presenting false testimony in court (something a person
who pleads guilty is unable to do) may have their sentences
increased.
178 Kim, “Underestimating the Trial Penalty,” 2015, 1245.
179 Or, put another way, sentences imposed following a plea were 39
percent shorter than those given following a trial. Ibid., 1246 and
1252, Table 3.
180 Ibid., 1254, Table 5.
181 Ulmer, Eisenstein, and Johnson, “Trial Penalties in Federal
Sentencing,” 2010, 575.
182 Kim, “Underestimating the Trial Penalty,” 2015, 1235; and Ulmer,
Eisenstein, and Johnson, “Trial Penalties in Federal Sentencing,”
2010, 570 and note 5.
183 Ulmer, Eisenstein, and Johnson, “Trial Penalties in Federal
Sentencing,” 2010, 575.
184 Besiki L. Kutateladze and Victoria Z. Lawson, “Is a Plea Really a
Bargain? An Analysis of Plea and Trial Dispositions in New York
City,” Crime and Delinquency 64, no. 7 (2018), 856-887; and David
S. Abrams, “Putting the Trial Penalty on Trial,” Duquesne Law Review
51 (2013), 777-785, https://perma.cc/886D-M3JV.
185 Kutateladze and Lawson, “Is a Plea Really A Bargain?”, 2018, 874.
186 Ibid., 866.
187 Ibid., 879.
188 Ibid.
189 Ibid., 877-878.
190 Ibid., 879.
64 Vera Institute of Justice
191 Ibid.
192 Abrams, “Putting the Trial Penalty on Trial,” 2013, 778. An earlier
study using data from 1977 similarly found that the trial penalty
disappeared when acquittals were taken into account. However,
the researcher noted that this would be true only for a hypothetical
“average” defendant or for cases in the aggregate, but not for any
one actual case. People are faced with the choice of potentially
being acquitted or receiving a more punitive sentence. LaFree,
Adversarial and Nonadversarial Justice,” 1985, 307.
193 Abrams, “Putting the Trial Penalty on Trial,” 2013, 782.
194 Ibid., 781. Although here it should be noted that Abrams did not
include a measure of oense severity other than the number of
charges each person faced. This means that the results cannot
discern whether people received longer sentences because they
went to trial or because they had committed more serious crimes.
See Kim, “Underestimating the Trial Penalty,” 2015, 1216.
195 Kim, “Underestimating the Trial Penalty,” 2015, 1201.
196 Albert W. Alschuler, “Lafler and Frye: Two Small Band-Aids for a
Festering Wound,” Duquesne Law Review 51 (2013), 673-707, 690-691,
https://perma.cc/PJ88-3KWD.
197 Kim, “Underestimating the Trial Penalty,” 2015, 1214.
198 Jed S. Rako, “Why Innocent People Plead Guilty,” New York
Review of Books, November 20, 2014, https://perma.cc/33UY-7PGE.
199 Blume and Helm, “The Unexonerated,” (2014), 157-191, 173.
200 Allison D. Redlich, Alicia Summers, and Steven Hoover, “Self-
Reported False Confessions and False Guilty Pleas Among
Oenders with Mental Illness,” Law and Human Behavior 34, no. 1
(2010), 79-90, 79 and 89.
201 Ibid., 80.
202 Rebecca K. Helm, Valerie F. Reyna, Allison A. Franz et al.,
“Limitations on the Ability to Negotiate Justice: Attorney
Perspectives on Guilt, Innocence, and Legal Advice in the Current
Plea System,” Psychology, Crime, & Law 24, no. 9 (2018), 915-934,
https://perma.cc/Q9KX-37ZS. Nearly 45 percent of the attorneys
involved in the study revealed that they had advised a client to take
a guilty plea despite believing that the client was innocent.
203 Redlich, Summers, and Hoover, “Self-Reported False Confessions,”
(2010), 79-90, 89.
204 Ibid., 87.
205 Lindsay C. Malloy, Elizabeth P. Shulman, and Elizabeth Cauman,
“Interrogations, Confessions, and Guilty Pleas Among Serious
Adolescent Oenders,” Law and Human Behavior 38, no. 2 (2014),
181-193, 189, https://perma.cc/P7EG-DUUA.
206 Ibid., 186.
207 Ibid., 189.
208 Samuel R. Gross, “Errors in Misdemeanor Adjudication,” Boston
University Law Review 98 (2018), 999-1011, 1005, https://perma.cc/
M3FX-2CR2.
209 Oren Gazal-Ayal and Avishalom Tor, “The Innocence Eect,” Duke
Law Journal 62 (2012), 339-401, 352, https://perma.cc/BZ22-NJ8D.
210 Gazal-Ayal and Tor, “The Innocence Eect,” 2012, 352.
211 Glinda S. Cooper, Vanessa Meterko, and Prahelika Gadtaula,
“Innocents Who Plead Guilty: An Analysis of Patterns in DNA
Exoneration Cases,” Federal Sentencing Reporter 31, no. 4-5 (2019),
234-238, 234, https://perma.cc/QV4G-B8FS.
212 Gazal-Ayal and Tor, “The Innocence Eect,” 2012, 352; and Cooper,
Meterko, and Gadtaula, “Innocents Who Plead Guilty,” 2019, 235.
213 Gross, “Errors in Misdemeanor Adjudication,” 2018, 1011.
214 Redlich, Summers, and Hoover, “Self-Reported False Confessions,”
2010, 80. Samuel R. Gross, Kristen Jacoby, Daniel J. Matheson et
al., “Exonerations in the United States 1989 through 2003,” Journal
of Criminal Law and Criminology 95, no. 2 (2005), 523, 536,
https://perma.cc/7LXF-7L5D; and Gross, “Errors in Misdemeanor
Adjudication,” 2018, 1011.
215 Gross, “Errors in Misdemeanor Adjudication,” 2018, 1003.
216 Ibid., 1000 (4 percent figure) and 1003 (Harris County).
217 Ibid., 1003.
218 Ibid., 1004. The author also notes that in 46 percent of cases in
which innocent people pled guilty to drug possession charges in
Harris County, the accused was Black—although only 24 percent
of Harris County population is Black. Ibid., 1009. This, the author
suggests, reflects the targeting of Black men by police when
stopping and searching people for drugs.
219 Ibid., 1004.
220 See Redlich, Summers, and Hoover, “Self-Reported False
Confessions,” 2010, 80.
221 Gross, “Errors in Misdemeanor Adjudication,” 2018, 1005.
222 Bibas, “Plea Bargaining Outside the Shadow of Trial,” 2004, 2463,
2465-2467 and 2496-2527.
223 Graham, “Crimes, Widgets, and Plea Bargaining,” 2012, 1573, 1583.
224 Ibid.
Acknowledgments
A special thank you to Nancy Fishman and Jim Parsons
for their insight and guidance throughout draing and
to Elizabeth Swavola and Akhi Johnson for reviewing
the report. ank you to Cindy Reed, Elle Teshima,
and Maris Mapolski for their hard work in the editing,
cite checking, and review processes; to Paragini
Amin for designing the report; and to Abbi Leman
for copyediting it. Finally, wed like to thank Laurie
Garduque, Patrick Griffin, and Maria Speiser of the
MacArthur Foundation for their support throughout
the development of this report.
is report was created with support from the John
D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation as part
of the Safe and Justice Challenge initiative, which
seeks to address overincarceration by changing
the way America thinks about and uses jails. Core
to the challenge is a grants competition designed to
support efforts to improve local criminal legal
systems in jurisdictions across the country. e
Foundation is supporting a nationwide network of
selected local jurisdictions commied to finding
ways to safely reduce jail incarceration—particularly
the disproportionate incarceration of racial and
ethnic minorities. More information is available at
.SafeandJusticeChallenge.org.
About citations
As researchers and readers alike rely more and
more on public knowledge made available through
the Internet, “link rot” has become a widely
acknowledged problem with creating usel and
sustainable citations. To address this issue, the Vera
Institute of Justice is experimenting with the use
of Perma.cc (hps://perma.cc/), a service that helps
scholars, journals, and courts create permanent links
to the online sources cited in their work.
Credits
© Vera Institute of Justice 2020. All rights reserved. An electronic version of this report is posted
on Vera’s website at www.vera.org/in-the-shadows.
Cover image: Geert Bollen / EyeEm via Getty Images
Graphics: Paragini Amin
For more information, visit www.vera.org.
For more information about this report, contact Léon Digard, research strategy editor, at
ldigard@vera.org. For more information about Vera’s work to reduce the use of jails, contact
Elizabeth Swavola, acting project director, Center on Sentencing and Corrections, at
eswavola@vera.org.
Suggested citation
Ram Subramanian, Léon Digard, Melvin Washington II, and Stephanie Sorage. In the Shadows:
A Review of the Research on Plea Bargaining. New York: Vera Institute of Justice, 2020.
This report was created with support from the John D.
and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation as part of the
Safety and Justice Challenge, which seeks to reduce over-
incarceration by changing the way America thinks about
and uses jails.
Vera Institute of Justice
34 35th Street, 4-2A
Brooklyn, NY 11232
T 212.334.1300
F 212.941.9407
New Orleans Oce
1307 Oretha Castle
Haley Blvd., Suite 203
New Orleans, LA 70113
T 504.312.4516
Los Angeles Oce
634 S Spring Street, #300A
Los Angeles, CA 90014
T 213.416.0058
F 213.416.0075
Washington DC Oce
1111 14th Street, Suite 920
Washington, DC 20005
T 202.465.8900
F 202.408.1972