HAL Id: hal-03039238
https://cnrs.hal.science/hal-03039238
Submitted on 7 Sep 2021
HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access
archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci-
entic research documents, whether they are pub-
lished or not. The documents may come from
teaching and research institutions in France or
abroad, or from public or private research centers.
L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est
destinée au dépôt et à la diusion de documents
scientiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non,
émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de
recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires
publics ou privés.
Theory-of-mind during childhood: Investigating
syntactic and executive contributions
Burnel Morgane, Durrleman Stéphanie, Reboul Anne, Carré Arnaud, Baciu
Monica, Perrone-bertolotti Marcela
To cite this version:
Burnel Morgane, Durrleman Stéphanie, Reboul Anne, Carré Arnaud, Baciu Monica, et al.. Theory-
of-mind during childhood: Investigating syntactic and executive contributions. Social Development,
2020, 30 (1), pp.73-94. �10.1111/sode.12471�. �hal-03039238�
Social Development. 2020;00:122.
|
1
wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/sode
Received: 5 June 2019 
|
  Revised: 30 June 2020 
|
  Accepted: 30 June 2020
DOI: 10.1111/sode.12471
ORIGINAL MANUSCRIPT
Theory-of-mind during childhood: Investigating
syntactic and executive contributions
Burnel Morgane
1,2
| Durrleman Stéphanie
3,4
| Reboul Anne
2
|
Carré Arnaud
5,6
| Baciu Monica
1,7
| Perrone-Bertolotti Marcela
1,7
This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and
reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
© 2020 The Authors. Social Development published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd
1
Univ. Grenoble Alpes, Univ. Savoie Mont
Blanc, CNRS, LPNC, Grenoble, France
2
Institute for Cognitive Sciences-Marc
Jeannerod, CNRS UMR 5304, Lyon, France
3
Department of Linguistics, Faculty of
Letters, University of Geneva, Geneva,
Switzerland
4
Psycholinguistics Department, Faculty
of Psychology and Educational Sciences,
University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
5
Univ. Grenoble Alpes, Univ. Savoie Mont
Blanc, LIP/PC2S, Grenoble, France
6
INSERM, Univ. Paris Descartes, Univ. Paris
Sud, Mental Health & Public Health-CESP
UMR 1018, Paris, France
7
Institut Universitaire de France, Paris,
France
Correspondence
Burnel Morgane, CNRS, LPNC UMR 5105,
University of Grenoble Alpes, F-38000
Grenoble, France.
Emil: morgane.burnel@univ-grenoble-
alpes.fr
Abstract
Both syntax and Executive Functions (EF) are involved in
Theory-of-Mind (ToM) but their contributory roles have
mainly been studied separately. Moreover, researchers have
mostly administered False Belief (FB) tasks while they may
not be representative of all ToM abilities. Studies of adults
give valuable information regarding whether syntax and
EF are useful for ToM reasoning (i.e., Reasoning account),
however, only the study of children brings direct evidence
in favor of ToM emergence (i.e., Emergence account). Also,
because the ToM tasks used often entail verbal and execu-
tive demands, the links observed could mostly result from
such confounds (i.e., Expression account). We evaluated
ToM, syntactic and EF abilities in 126 children (3-11 y.o.)
using a set of ToM tasks with minimal verbal and executive
demands. Our goals were to assess (1) the hierarchical con-
tribution of syntax and EF to ToM, (2) whether results pre-
viously obtained for FB tasks are representative of ToM in
general, (3) whether the ToM-syntax and ToM-EF links are
constant (i.e., Reasoning account) or decrease during devel-
opment (i.e., Emergence accounts). Results of stepwise re-
gression analyses showed a predominant role of syntax over
EF to predict ToM abilities. The comparison of results for
ToM and FB tasks showed that FB is not always representa-
tive of ToM. Finally, there was no moderating effect of age
2 
|
  
MORGANE Et Al.
1 | INTRODUCTION
Theory-of-Mind (ToM) is the ability to attribute mental states to others in order to predict or explain their behavior
(Premack & Woodruff, 1978). ToM is mainly assessed by means of False Belief (FB) tasks. The most famous FB task
is the Sally-Anne task (Baron-Cohen, Leslie, & Frith, 1985) where children are asked to predict where Sally will look
for her ball after Anne has moved it in Sally’s absence from the basket, where Sally had left it, to the box. Children
start to predict Sallys behavior according to her belief rather than according to their own knowledge of reality
only after the age of 4 or 5 years (Yirmiya, Erel, Shaked, & Solomonica-Levi, 1998). In doing so, they demonstrate
that they are aware of the existence of mental states guiding others’ behavior, that is, to say they demonstrate
their ability to use ToM (Dennett, 1978).
1.1 | Hierarchical contribution of syntax and executive functions (EF) to ToM
Theory-of-Mind development is related to a set of cognitive skills, especially linguistic skills (Farrar, Benigno,
Tompkins, & Gage, 2017; Milligan, Astington, & Dack, 2007; Tompkins, Farrar, & Montgomery, 2019) and execu-
tive functions (for a Meta-analysis see Devine & Hughes, 2014).
General language (e.g., Farrar et al., 2009; Tager-Flusberg & Joseph, 2005), receptive vocabulary (e.g., Carlson
& Moses, 2001) and mental state lexicon (e.g., Schick, de Villiers, de Villiers, & Hoffmeister, 2007) with syntactic
embedding play an important role in the typical development of ToM (Durrleman, Burnel, de Villiers, Thommen,
Yan & Delage, 2019; Hale & Tager-Flusberg, 2003; Milligan et al., 2007; Poltrock, 2011; for an alternate view see
Farrar et al., 2017). In particular, studies have found a link between both mastery of Relative Clause Sentences
(RCS) and Complement Sentences (CS) and FB success (Durrleman, Hinzen & Franck, 2018; Smith, Apperly &
White, 2003). CS are complex sentences that allow the embedding of a false proposition into a true one. For
example, in “Sally thinks that the marble is in the basket,the underlined embedded proposition can be false with
the entire sentence being true. Relative Clause Sentences (RCS), such as “Sally plays with the marble that is in the
basket,are other complex sentences that also allow the embedding of a proposition into another. Nevertheless,
contrary to CS, if the embedded proposition is false (i.e., in our example if there is no marble in the basket) the
entire RCS is false too. Thus, CS, but not RCS, are particularly well suited to reason that someone is misrepresent-
ing reality. Thus, de Villiers and de Villiers (2000) proposed that CS is a useful tool for representing others’ (false)
mental states (for a meta-analysis, see Milligan et al., 2007). CS can occur with different types of verbs, such as
cognition verbs (e.g., think, believe), but also communication verbs (e.g., say, tell) that do not imply the semantics
of mental states. Complements of communication verbs have been claimed to be indicative of a purer measure of
complementation abilities precisely because they do not imply mental state concepts, removing this confounding
variable with ToM. Studies of typically developing children showed that complementation with communication
verbs is related to FB performance (de Villiers & de Villiers, 2012; de Villiers & Pyers, 2002; Durrleman & Franck,
2015; Hale & Tager-Flusberg, 2003; Low, 2010; Ng, Cheung, & Xiao, 2010), and that complementation training
improves false belief understanding (Durrleman et al., 2019; Lohmann & Tomasello, 2003; Sellabona et al., 2013).
A limitation of previous studies is that CS understanding has been assessed by means of complements that were
on the syntax-ToM or EF-ToM relations, thus suggestive of
the Reasoning account rather than the Emergence account.
KEYWORDS
executive functions, false belief, syntax, theory-of-mind
  
|
 3
MORGANE Et Al.
explicitly false. However, Sellabona et al. (2013) showed that the training of false complements, but not that of
true complements, improved the ability to succeed at FB tasks. They conclude that falsity, rather than embed-
ded syntax, fosters children’s understanding of FB. Comparing CS to RCS allows teasing apart (i) the impact of
syntactic complexity, also associated with RCS and (ii) the misrepresentation properties specific to CS. To our
knowledge, only Hale and Tager-Flusberg (2003) compared CS and RCS contribution to FB. Their results suggest
a major role of CS for FB compared to the role of RCS.
ToM skills have also been related to EF (Devine & Hughes, 2014; Pineda-Alhucema, Aristizabal, Escudero-
Cabarcas, Acosta-Lopez, & Vélez, 2018). However, it is currently difficult to extract from the literature a particular
executive component that would be the most related to ToM. Inhibition has been envisaged as one main executive
skill required for success on ToM tasks (e.g., Brock, Kim, Gutshall, & Grissmer, 2018; Flynn, 2007) because children
need to be able to suppress their own perspective to accurately grasp others’ views and knowledge. In addition,
neuroimaging studies in adults have shown that the neural bases of ToM and inhibition partially overlap (Mahy,
Moses, & Pfeifer, 2014). According to Rothmayr et al. (2010) and van der Meer et al. (2011), these could be similar
in FB tasks and inhibition tasks (like stop signal tasks) in the bilateral inferior and middle frontal gyrus, the right me-
dial and inferior frontal and temporal gyrii, as well as the temporo-parietal junction. Beyond the involvement of in-
hibition, flexibility, that is to say the ability to shift between tasks or task-rules in order to produce a goal-adapted
behavior, has also been proposed as a useful ability to succeed in ToM tasks (e.g., Lewis-Morrarty, Dozier, Bernard,
Terracciano, & Moore, 2012) since it allows an individual to shift between conflicting perspectives. The ability to
maintain and manipulate information during a short period of time, that is to say Working Memory (WM), is also
arguably an important element for ToM (e.g., Austin, Groppe, & Elsner, 2014) as it allows children to maintain
crucial information available (e.g., mental states, reality) while using it to predict or explain someone’s behavior.
Despite the fact that in the literature both syntax and EF appear to be related to ToM, few studies evaluated
them together (Brock et al., 2018; Carlson, Moses, & Claxton, 2004; Durrleman & Franck, 2015; Hughes, 1998;
Hughes & Ensor, 2007; Marcovitch et al., 2015). Those that did, however, were primarily focused on ToM-EF
links controlling for verbal skills, mostly lexical abilities, and consequently do not yield insights on the potential
hierarchical contribution of syntactic and executive abilities on ToM. Only the study from Durrleman and Franck
(2015) revealed a predominant contribution of syntax over EF in FB attribution. However, their results should be
taken with caution given the small sample size (n = 17). The first goal of the current study was thus to assess the
hierarchical contribution of syntax and EF to ToM abilities in children using a substantial sample size.
1.2 | The common use of FB tasks to assess ToM
In children, ToM is mostly studied through FB tasks (Smith & Wu, 2016). These tasks were proposed by Dennett
(1978) as the acid test of ToM. Nevertheless, several authors suggested that ToM is not limited to FB understand-
ing (Bloom & German, 2000; Wellman, Cross, & Watson, 2001; Wellman & Liu, 2004) and FB tasks may not be rep-
resentative of general ToM abilities. For instance, in a review, Saxe, Carey and Kanwisher (2004) provide evidence
that the neural substrates associated with belief attribution are distinct from those observed during ToM tasks
like goal attribution. In light of this, the massive and almost exclusive use of FB tasks to study ToM is an important
limitation to the understanding of global ToM development.
Furthermore, the literature often fails to clearly specify whether the formulated hypothesis concerns ToM in
general or solely FB. We argue that, while FB reasoning is an interesting domain of investigation, the study of ToM
cannot be limited to FB attribution because conclusions based on FB tasks may not be generalizable to ToM. Many
researchers may wish to understand the predictors of ToM other than FB, so as to allow the implementation of
remediation programs designed to promote social skills relying on the mastery of ToM abilities other than belief
attribution. To increase the potential of such a program, findings beyond FB attribution need to be considered.
In particular, even though training on CS is efficient to promote FB understanding, this could be too specific to
4 
|
  
MORGANE Et Al.
improve other ToM abilities, and all the more social skills in daily life. Promoting FB reasoning alone might not be
enough to yield wider social benefits, which is of paramount importance for clinical populations where ToM is
delayed, such as children with autism spectrum disorders.
In the current study, we examined how syntax and EF are related to ToM development, beyond FB attribution.
The second goal was to thus assess if results previously obtained for FB tasks are representative of results ob-
tained for more general ToM abilities.
1.3 | Expression, emergence, and reasoning accounts
Regardless of the specific cognitive abilities studied (e.g., language, EF), three hypotheses can be proposed to
explain their relation to ToM (cf. Figure 1). According to the Expression account, low ToM performance does not
necessarily correspond to low ToM competence, but could also reflect good ToM competence with constraints in
the specific task used that prevent the child from succeeding. For example, additional linguistic (Apperly, Samson,
& Humphreys, 2009; Craven, 2005) or executive demands (Moses, 2001) coming from task presentation or in-
structions could restrain ToM performance despite good ToM competence. The Expression account can be consid-
ered in a strong and in a weak version (Craven, 2005). In the strong version, the links with ToM are considered as
entirely determined by task demands. In the weak version, even though ToM can be correlated to other cognitive
abilities due to additional demands coming from task presentation, a more fundamental link nevertheless exists.
In their meta-analysis, Wellman et al. (2001) compared children’s success at FB tasks depending on task features.
For example, they found that FB tasks were succeeded at a younger age when children were explicitly told that
there was a deceptive motive for the change of location (i.e., salience of mental state), when the moved object
FIGURE 1 The Reasoning, strong Emergence, and weak Emergence accounts are subordinated to the weak
Expression account which compete with the strong Expression account. The three graphics at the bottom
represent the evolution of the links between ToM and other cognitive abilities depending on age: wider parts of
black shapes represent more important relations
  
|
 5
MORGANE Et Al.
is less salient (i.e., diminishing the need to inhibit its location, for example, when Maxi eats the chocolate rather
than moves it), or when the question specifically refers to the protagonist mental states (i.e.,Where Sally thinks
that her marble is?” rather than “Where will Sally go to find her marble?”). Nevertheless, some studies on typically
developing children showed that the ToM-language (Durrleman et al., 2016) or ToM-EF links (Carlson et al., 1998;
Moses, 2001) are not due to task demands only and continue to exist when low-verbal or low-executive ToM tasks
are used (i.e., in favor of a weak version of the Expression account).
Subordinated to the weak Expression account, two categories of hypotheses can be proposed in order to un-
derstand the fundamental links between ToM and other cognitive abilities such as syntax and specific EF (cf.
Figure 1).
Moreover, the Reasoning account postulates that certain cognitive abilities are necessary to reason about ToM
(Apperly et al., 2009), while the Emergence account instead maintains that these abilities are only necessary to
enable ToM development (Moses, 2001). Thus, the Reasoning account predicts constant links between ToM and
syntax or EF throughout the lifespan, and the Emergence account predicts weaker links when growing older. In
addition, these links could diminish until disappearance at adulthood (i.e., strong Emergence account) or they could
diminish while remaining present in adults (i.e., weak Emergence account).
Importantly, the strong Expression account, if validated, would nullify both the Reasoning and Emergence ac-
counts. By contrast, the weak Expression account is compatible with both Reasoning and Emergence accounts.
Indeed, language or EF could limit ToM Expression and at the same time they could be useful either for ToM
Reasoning or for ToM Emergence (cf. Figure 1). There is currently no doubt that linguistic and executive demands
can affect ToM performance (for a meta-analysis see Wellman et al., 2001). While ToM tasks that are completely
void of such demands are probably impossible to create, one must strive nevertheless to use tasks that minimize
such demands so as to tease apart Reasoning and Emergence accounts. Without this, there can be no guarantee
that the links identified between ToM and other cognitive abilities could not be solely attributed to the Expression
account (strong version).
Expression and Emergence accounts were firstly proposed by Moses (2001) regarding the ToM-EF links. Miller
(2001) made a similar proposal regarding ToM-syntax links but with different terminology (i.e., weak and strong
versions of the linguistic determinism hypothesis of de Villiers & de Villiers, 2000). We propose to consider three
theories incorporating the various views put forth in the literature regarding ToM-EF and ToM-language relations
articulated as follows: Expression account (weak and strong versions), Emergence account (weak and strong versions)
and Reasoning account (see also Apperly et al., 2009; Craven, 2005).
Studies in adults give valuable information regarding the Reasoning vs. Emergence accounts. In particular, they
allow distinguishing between the Reasoning account that predicts the persistence of links between ToM and other
cognitive abilities in adults, and the strong Emergence account that predicts their absence. Overall, studies on adult s
suggest that language, and in particular syntax, is not clearly implied in ToM beyond childhood (Dodell-Feder,
Koster-Hale, Bedny, & Saxe, 2011; Saxe et al., 2004). Indeed, brain-injured adults with aphasia are able to complete
FB tasks even when unable to produce or understand syntactic structures such as complement sentences (e.g.,
Apperly, Samson, Carroll, Hussain, & Humphreys, 2006; Siegal, Varley, & Want, 2001). Dual-task studies in healthy
adults showed that even though being involved in a concurrent linguistic task decreases performance at a FB task,
this interference is specific neither to linguistic interference nor to ToM performance (Burnel, Perrone-Bertolotti,
Durrleman, Reboul, & Baciu, 2017; Forgeot d’Arc & Ramus, 2011). The literature of ToM-syntax links in adults
has, thus, showed indirect evidence in favor of the strong Emergence account. By contrast, studies of ToM-EF
links in adults reported ToM impairments isolated from executive impairments (Fine, Lumsden, & Blair, 2001) or
the co-occurrence of ToM and executive deficits (Henry, Phillips, Crawford, Ietswaart, & Summers, 2006), but
no case of a patient exhibiting EF impairments without ToM impairments (see also Bull, Phillips, & Conway, 2008
for a dual-task study in healthy adults). Thus, the literature of ToM-EF links suggests that EF are still implicated
in ToM at adulthood. Nevertheless, both the Reasoning and weak Emergence accounts predict the persistence of
ToM-EF links in adults. Only the study of children with a wide age range allows distinguishing the Reasoning and
6 
|
  
MORGANE Et Al.
weak Emergence accounts. However, until now and to the best of our knowledge, there is no study evaluating these
accounts in a population of typically developing children, neither regarding the ToM-syntax links nor regarding the
ToM-EF links. Using a method that has been seldom used in the field, the third goal of the current study was thus
to compare the Reasoning and Emergence accounts (i.e., weak and strong versions) by examining age as a moderator
variable for the relation between ToM and syntax or EF.
1.4 | Goals of the current study
To summarize, the main goal of the current study is to determine the most important components for ToM amongst
syntactic and executive abilities. Past studies examining the development of ToM abilities have been limited by
relying exclusively on FB tasks as the sole indicator of ToM. These tasks may themselves present syntactic and
executive demands, which limit their use for teasing apart the accounts we are concerned with. Both syntax and
EF are reported as involved in ToM, however, studies evaluating ToM-syntax and ToM-EF relations leave many
questions open: What specific syntactic or executive components are involved in ToM? Is there a priority between
syntactic and executive abilities for ToM Reasoning or Emergence? Which of the Reasoning or Emergence accounts
best explains the findings?
Using a set of ToM tasks with minimal verbal and executive demands in order to guarantee that the links identi-
fied could not be solely attributed to the strong Expression account, the first goal of the current study was to assess
the hierarchical contribution of syntax and EF to ToM abilities in children. Using non-verbal FB tasks, the second
goal was to assess if results previously obtained for FB tasks are representative of results obtained for a broader
set of ToM tasks. In studying children aged between 3 and 11 years old, the third goal was to assess whether the
ToM-syntax and ToM-EF links are constant (i.e., Reasoning account) or decrease during development (i.e., strong
or weak Emergence accounts).
2 | METHOD
2.1 | Participants
One hundred and twenty-six French-speaking children (60 boys and 66 girls) participated in the study. They were
between 3.2 and 11.9 years old (mean age = 7.0, SD = 2.3), with a 25th percentile of 5.10 years, a 50th percentile
of 6.60 years and a 75th percentile of 9.10 years (see Table 1). Children were recruited in two schools in the region
of Grenoble, France. Parents gave written consent for their child to participate in the study. The experimental
procedure was approved by the local ethics committee for non-interventional research, Pole Grenoble Cognition
(N° IRB00010290-2016-01-05-01).
2.2 | Procedure
Children were seen in a quiet room in their school by one of four trained experimenters. The total duration of the
experiment varied between 1 hr and 1 hr and a half, depending on how fast children answered. The tasks were
divided into three blocks. The order of blocks was counterbalanced across participants and children completed
each block in a different session. The study was completed during three test sessions, with a mean gap of three
days between the first and last sessions (range 0−28 days). The six experimental tasks are summarized in Figure 2
and detailed below.
  
|
 7
MORGANE Et Al.
TABLE 1 Descriptive data for age, low-verbal Theory of Mind (ToM) scale, False Belief (FB), Complement
Sentences (CS), Relative Clause Sentences (RCS), inhibition, flexibility, and Working Memory (WM) depending on
age group (All ages, 3 to 5 years old, 6 to 8 years old, and 9 to 11 years old)
Age group Age ToM scale FB CS RCS Inhibition Flexibility WM
N All 126 126 125 126 126 125 118 124
3-5 y. o. 50 50 49 50 50
6-8 y. o. 41 41 41 41 41
9-11 y. o. 35 35 35 35 35
Mean All 7.0 4 3.21 4.41 6.73 7.18 0.302 3.93 71.6
3-5 y. o. 4.73 2.06 3.39 4.26 4.96 0.468 1.95 60.3
6-8 y. o. 7.4 4 3.68 4.95 7.83 8.39 0.234 5.20 80.5
9-11 y. o. 9.86 4.31 5.20 8.97 8.94 0.138 5.01 77.4
Median All 6.60 3.00 4 8.00 8.00 0.200 3.11 74.0
3-5 y. o. 4.70 2.00 3 4.00 5.00 0.450 1.66 62
6-8 y. o. 7. 3 0 4 5 9 9 0.200 6.04 83.5
9-11 y. o. 9.50 5 6 10 9 0.100 4.10 78
Standard
deviation
All 2.27 1.44 2.10 3.33 2.51 0.275 2.66 17.7
3-5 y. o. 0.746 1.02 1.66 2.59 2.17 0.305 1.79 17.4
6-8 y. o. 1.05 1.29 1.80 3.07 1.50 0.192 2.46 14.0
9-11 y. o. 0.757 0.832 2.44 2.12 1.21 0.160 2.33 12.7
Minimum All 3.20 0 1 0 1 0.000 0.0600 18
3-5 y. o. 3.20 0 1 0 1 0.000 0.0600 18
6-8 y. o. 6.00 0 2 0 5 0.000 2.07 38
9-11 y. o. 9.00 2 1 1 6 0.000 2.07 53
Maximum
possible
5 8 10 10 105
Maximum All 12.0 5 8 10 10 1.10 11.0 101
3-5 y. o. 5.90 4 7 10 10 1.10 9.06 96
6-8 y. o. 8.90 5 8 10 10 0.700 11.0 101
9-11 y. o. 12.0 5 8 10 10 0.600 11.0 98
25th percentile All 5.10 2.00 3.00 4.00 5.00 0.100 2.09 60.0
3-5 y. o. 4.13 1.00 2.00 3.00 4.00 0.300 0.310 50.0
6-8 y. o. 6.40 3.00 3.00 7.0 0 8.00 0.100 3.08 71.0
9-11 y. o. 9.25 4.00 3.00 9.00 8.00 0.0250 3.10 68.0
50th percentile All 6.60 3.00 4.00 8.00 8.00 0.200 3.11 74.0
3-5 y. o. 4.70 2.00 3.00 4.00 5.00 0.450 1.66 62.0
6-8 y. o. 7. 3 0 4.00 5.00 9.00 9.00 0.200 6.04 83.5
9-11 y. o. 9.50 5.00 6.00 10.0 9.0 0 0.100 4.10 78.0
75th percentile All 9.10 4.75 6.00 10.0 9.00 0.500 6.06 85.3
3-5 y. o. 5.38 3.00 4.00 6.00 6.00 0.675 3.09 72.0
6-8 y. o. 8.60 5.00 6.00 10.0 9.0 0 0.400 7.05 90.5
9-11 y. o. 10.5 5.00 7.0 0 10.0 10.0 0.200 7. 0 4 88.0
8 
|
  
MORGANE Et Al.
2.3 | Material
2.3.1| Low-verbal ToM scale
ToM was assessed by means of the low-verbal ToM scale adapted by Burnel, Perrone-Bertolotti, Baciu, Reboul,
and Durrleman (2018) from Wellman and Liu (2004). This tool evaluates ToM development via five subtasks (see
FIGURE 2 Theory-of-mind (ToM) was assessed by mean of a low-verbal ToM scale (Burnel et al., 2018)
including five tasks (i.e., Diverse Desire, Diverse Belief, Explicit False Belief, Content False Belief, and Hidden
Emotion) and by means of a non-verbal False Belief task including eight trials (Forgeot dArc & Ramus, 2011).
One task evaluated syntactic abilities (i.e., complement sentences or relative clause sentences) and the remaining
three tasks assessed three executive components: inhibition (Wright et al., 2003), flexibility (verbal fluency task),
and working memory (Poncelet & Van Der Linden, 2003)
TABLE 2 The five subtasks and corresponding Theory-of-mind abilities assessed in the low-verbal scale
Subtask name Theory-of-mind abilities assessed
Diverse Desires Children understand that people will act according to their own desires
Diverse Beliefs Children understand that people will act according to their own beliefs
Explicit FB Children link the search behavior of a character to his (explicit) false belief
Contents FB Children attribute a false belief to a character
Hidden Emotion Children understand that emotions felt can be different from emotion shown
  
|
 9
MORGANE Et Al.
Table 2), with a 0.93 Green’s coefficient of reproducibility (Burnel et al., 2018). The total number of correct an-
swers was recorded, with a maximum score of 5. The total duration of the task was 15 to 30 min.
2.3.2 | Non-verbal FB task
The non-verbal FB task consisted of eight silent cartoons selected from those proposed by Forgeot d’Arc and
Ramus (2011). This task was designed to assess FB understanding based on various paradigms (i.e., appearance-
reality, change of location, and reality unknown). It was tested in typically developing children aged between 4 and
7 years (Forgeot dArc, 2009), in deaf children (Levrez et al., 2012) and in adults (Forgeot dArc & Ramus, 2011).
For example, one cartoon shows two plants, one on a yellow table and another on a red table (see Figure 3). A boy
starts watering the plant on the yellow table and then goes to fill his watering can. While he is not watching, a girl
enters the scene and swaps the two plants. Then the boy turns around and is standing between the two plants.
Children participating in the study are asked at this point to choose if the boy will water the plant on the yellow
table (i.e., incorrect answer) or on the red table (i.e., correct answer). The two possible endings were displayed on
the screen at the same time. Children responded by pointing to the correct answer. The total number of correct
answers was recorded, with a maximum score of 8. The total duration of the task was 15 to 20 min.
2.3.3 | Syntactic tasks
Syntactic tasks were proposed in order to evaluate the understanding of two embedded structures. The 10 CS and
10 RCS were constructed with similar vocabulary and they mainly differed in terms of their syntactic construction.
For example, the characters sheep” and “cat” were used with the action “playing with the ball” in a CS (i.e., “The
cat says that the sheep is playing with the ball”) and in a RCS (i.e., “The cat bites the sheep that is playing with the
ball”) but with a different verb for the main clause (e.g., “says” vs “bites”). For each sentence, we proposed a quad-
ruplet of pictures varying the agent of the principal and embedded clauses (cf. Figure 4). For example, four pictures
illustrated (1) “the cat bites the sheep that is playing with the ball,” (2) “The sheep bites the cat that is playing with
the ball,” (3) “The cat that is playing with the ball bites the sheep,” and (4) “The sheep that is playing with the ball
bites the cat.” Children were instructed to judge which picture among four best matched the sentence they heard
(see Figure 4). Each correct answer was worth 1 point, with a maximum score of 10 per sentence type. The total
duration of the task was 10 min.
FIGURE 3 Participants see a boy watering the plant on the left of the screen. While the boy is filling his
water can and not watching, a girl enters the scene and swaps the two plants. Then the boy comes back
between the two plants. The participant is finally asked to choose if the boy waters the plant on the left of the
screen (i.e., incorrect answer) or the plant on the right of the screen (i.e., correct answer)
10 
|
  
MORGANE Et Al.
2.3.4 | Inhibition task
Inhibition was evaluated by means of an adapted version of the Stroop-like measure proposed by Wright,
Waterman, Prescott, and Murdoch-Eaton (2003) using 24 congruent and 24 incongruent pictures of animals (see
Figure 5). Individual interference indices (i.e., difference between mean reaction times for congruent and incon-
gruent stimuli) were computed and used as measures of inhibition
1
. The total duration of the task was 5 min.
2.3.5 | Flexibility task
Flexibility was assessed by a semantic fluency task. Children were instructed to name as many animals as they
could in 1 min. We computed the number of relative switches (i.e., total number of switches between semantic
categories divided by the total number of correct words) as an indicator of flexibility (Begeer et al., 2014; Troyer,
Moscovitch, & Winocur, 1997). All answers were coded by two trained experimenters with an inter-rater agree-
ment of 91% resolved to 100% after discussion.
2.3.6 | Working memory task
In order to assess the phonological stock of WM, children participated in a pseudo-word repetition task adapted
to French by Poncelet and Van Der Linden (2003). They were instructed to repeat pronounceable pseudo-words
presented in the auditory modality. The total number of syllables correctly repeated was computed, with a maxi-
mum score of 105 correct syllables per child.
2.4 | Data scoring and statistical analyses
Because data were non-normally distributed we used non-parametric statistics. When non-parametric statistics
were not available (i.e., for stepwise regression analyses and moderation analyses), parametric statistics were
performed on transformed data using Box-Cox transformations (Osborne, 2010). In reporting the results, we
follow Wilkinson (1999) recommendations and report exact p values (unless p < .001) and effect sizes. We used
FIGURE 4 Participants choose between four pictures the one corresponding to the sentence presented
Complement Sentences Relative Clause Sentences
‘Le chat raconte que le mouton joue au ballon’
The cat says that the sheep is playing with the ball
‘Le chat mord le mouton qui joue au ballon’
The cat bites the sheep that is playing with the ball
  
|
 11
MORGANE Et Al.
studentized deleted residuals analyses to identify outliers (i.e., values greater than 3.0). When data were detected
as outliers they were discarded.
The total sample size was 126 children. Missing data varied between 0 and 8 depending on tasks and analyses.
Participants were included in multiple regression analyses only if they had complete data for the set of variables
analyzed.
Firstly, we assessed the suitability of our tasks for the age range studied. In order to do so we present descrip-
tive data (see Table 1) for the entire sample but also for age groups (i.e., 3-5 years old, 6-8 years old, and 9-11 years
old), even though age is not treated categorically in this study.
Secondly, in order to assess whether performance on tasks changes with children’s age we evaluated their
correlations with Spearman’s rank correlation tests (r
s
). After that, to examine if syntactic and executive variables
were related to ToM or FB beyond their common relation to age, we evaluated their correlations controlling for
age with partial correlation analyses.
Thirdly, we ranked the contribution of syntactic and executive variables to ToM scores by means of stepwise
multiple regression analysis with forward selection. This analysis allowed us to spot syntactic and executive com-
ponents that are specifically linked to ToM. The same stepwise multiple regression analysis was then conducted
with Non-verbal FB scores as the dependent variable. This analysis allowed us to spot specific syntactic and ex-
ecutive components that are specifically linked to non-verbal FB, and thus to compare our results with previous
literature.
Fourth, we tested children’s age as a possible moderator of the syntax-ToM and EF-ToM relations by means of
moderation analyses, so as to assess the Emergence and Reasoning accounts. For ease of comparison with previous
literature, the same moderation analyses were also run for non-verbal FB scores.
3 | RESULTS
3.1 | Descriptive data and suitability of tasks
The complete descriptive data on tasks are available in Table 1. Percentiles are provided for the full sample
(3-11 years old) but also depending on age groups (3-5 years old, 6-8 years old, and 9-11 years old). It is worth
noting that all the statistical analyses were conducted in the full sample. Thus, the suitability of tasks should be
studied by considering percentiles in the full sample (3-11 years old). We consider that there is a ceiling effect for
a task if more than 25% of children obtained the maximum score.
FIGURE 5 Participants name animals according to their body in congruent and incongruent stimuli
12 
|
  
MORGANE Et Al.
Average performance was 3.21 on the low-verbal ToM scale. In the entire sample, 50th percentile was 3, and
75th percentile was 4.75, near the maximum score of 5. Children aged between 3 and 5 years old did not perform
at ceiling (75th percentile = 3.00) contrary to 6-8 years old (75th percentile = 5.00) and 9-11 years old (50th
percentile = 5.00).
For FB tasks, average performance was 4.41 for a maximum score of 8. Only nine children performed at ceiling.
At 75th percentile was 6 for the entire sample, 4 for 3-5 years old, 6 for 6-8 years old and 7 for 9-11 years old.
CS and RCS scores were on average of 6.73 and 7.18 for a maximum score of 10.
In the entire sample, 75th percentile was 10 for CS and 9 for RCS. In particular, children aged between 3 and
5 years old did not perform at ceiling on CS or on RCS (75th percentile = 6), 6-8 years old were at ceiling for CS
(75th percentile = 10) but not RCS (75th percentile = 9), and 9-11 years old children performed at ceiling for both
CS (50th percentile = 10) and RCS (75th percentile = 10).
Regarding the inhibition task, the average score was 0.30. 12% of children (i.e., 15/126) achieved ceiling per-
formance of 0 (i.e., no difference between congruent and incongruent items). The 75th percentile was 0.50 in the
entire sample, 0.68 for 3-5 years old children, 0.40 for 6-8 years old children and 0.20 for 9-11 years old.
The mean relative number of switches in the flexibility task was 3.93 for the entire sample, with a 75th percen-
tile at 6.06. There was no maximum for the number of correct words and switches between semantic categories,
and thus for the flexibility task.
Average scores at the WM task were 71.6, with all children scoring below the maximum of 105. The 75th per-
centile was 85.3 in the entire sample, 72.0 for 3-5 years old children, 90.5 in 6-8 years old and 88.0 in 6-11 years
old.
3.2 | Correlations between tasks
According to Spearman’s correlations (see Table 3), age was significantly correlated to low-verbal ToM scale
(r
S
(124) = 0.70, p < .001), non-verbal FB (r
S
(123) = 0.38, p < .001), CS (r
s
(124) = 0.68, p < .001), RCS (r
s
(124) = 0.73,
p < .001), inhibition (r
s
(123) = −0.49, p < .001), flexibility (r
s
(116) = 0.58, p < .001) and WM (r
s
(122) = 0.45, p < .001).
The shared variance between age and the other variables (i.e., Low-verbal ToM scale, non-verbal FB, CS, RCS,
inhibition, flexibility, and WM) varied between 14% (i.e., FB) and 73% (i.e., RCS).
After controlling for the effect of age (see Table 4), the low-verbal ToM scale was significantly correlated to
CS (r
s
(124) = 0.34, p < .001), RCS (r
s
(124) = 0.28, p < .001), flexibility (r
s
(116) = 0.20, p < .001) and WM (r
s
(122) =
0.23, p < .001), with a shared variance varying between 4% (i.e., flexibility) and 12% (i.e., CS). Nevertheless, the
correlation between the low-verbal ToM scale and inhibition that was statistically significant (r
s
(123) = −0.32, p <
.001) became statistically non-significant after controlling for the effect of age (r
s
(123) = −0.04, p = .51).
After controlling for the effect of age (see Table 4), non-verbal FB was also significantly correlated to CS
(r
S
(123) = 0.16, p = .008), RCS (r
S
(123) = 0.22, p < .001), flexibility (r
S
(115) = 0.15, p = .02) and WM (r
S
(121) = 0.18,
p = .004), with a shared variance varying between 2% (i.e., flexibility) and 5% (i.e., RCS). Nevertheless, the cor-
relation between non-verbal FB and inhibition that was statistically significant (r
S
(122) = −0.20, p = .02) became
statistically non-significant after controlling for the effect of age (r
S
(122) = −0.04, p = .55).
3.3 | Stepwise multiple regression analyses
A stepwise multiple regression analysis was conducted to assess whether age, CS, RCS, inhibition, flexibility, and
WM were useful to predict low-verbal ToM scale performances (see Table 5). At step 1 of the analysis, age en-
tered into the regression equation and was significantly related to the low-verbal ToM scale (F(1, 115) = 98.5, p <
.001). The multiple determination coefficient was .46, indicating that approximately 46% of low-verbal ToM scale
  
|
 13
MORGANE Et Al.
performance could be accounted for by age. At step 2, CS entered the model and was significantly related to low-
verbal ToM scale (F(2, 114) = 56.0, p < .001), increasing the total variance explained from 0.46 to 0.50, an increase
of 0.04. By itself, CS explained 34% of the variance in low-verbal ToM scale performance, but a portion of what it
could explain had already been attributed to age. At step 3, WM entered the model and was significantly related to
the low-verbal ToM scale (F(3, 113) = 39.7, p < .001), increasing the total variance explained from 0.50 to 0.51, an
increase of 0.01. By itself, WM explained 19% of the variance in low-verbal ToM scale performance, but a portion
of what it could explain had already been attributed to age and CS. No other variable entered into the equation at
step 4 of the analysis. Thus, the regression equation for predicting low-verbal ToM scale performance was: Low-
verbal ToM scale
1.39
= 0.71 × Age + 0.23 × CS + 0.03 × WM 1.17.
A stepwise multiple regression analysis was conducted to evaluate whether age, CS, RCS, inhibition, flexibility,
and WM were useful to predict non-verbal FB performances (see Table 5). At step 1 of the analysis, RCS entered
into the regression equation and were significantly related to non-verbal FB (F(1, 114) = 28.5, p < .001). The multi-
ple determination coefficient was 0.20, indicating that approximately 20% of non-verbal FB performance could be
TABLE 3 Results of Spearman’s rank correlations between age and tasks for low-verbal Theory-of-Mind
(ToM) scale, False Belief (FB), Complement Sentences (CS), Relative Clause Sentences (RCS), inhibition, flexibility,
and Working Memory (WM)
Age
ToM
scale FB CS RCS Inhibition Flexibility WM
Age
ToM scale 0.701*
FB 0.396* 0.416*
CS 0.675* 0.599* 0.365*
RCS 0.720* 0.599* 0.418* 0.704*
Inhibition −0.509* 0.316* 0.200* 0.371* 0.469*
Flexibility 0.581* 0.484* 0.342* 0.425* 0.508* −0.343*
WM 0.474* 0.465* 0.362* 0.376* 0.535* 0.252* 0.437*
*p < .05.
TABLE 4 Results of Spearman’s rank correlations controlling for age on low-verbal Theory-of-Mind (ToM)
scale, False Belief (FB), Complement Sentences (CS), Relative Clause Sentences (RCS), inhibition, flexibility, and
Working Memory (WM)
ToM
scale FB CS RCS Inhibition Flexibility WM
ToM scale
FB 0.218*
CS 0.229* 0.169
RCS 0.177 0.230* 0.439*
Inhibition 0.022 0.018 −0.066 0.329*
Flexibility 0.157 0.180 0.065 0.329 0.115
WM 0.194* 0.180 0.083 0.329* −0.063 0.245*
*p < .05.
14 
|
  
MORGANE Et Al.
accounted for by RCS. At step 2, flexibility entered the model and was significantly related to non-verbal FB (F(2,
113) = 17.0, p < .001), increasing the total variance explained from 0.20 to 0.23, an increase of 0.03. By itself, flex-
ibility explained 14% of the variance in non-verbal FB performance. No other variable entered into the equation
at step 3 of the analysis. Thus, the regression equation for predicting non-verbal FB performance was: Non-verbal
FB
.79
= 0.16 × RCS + 0.09 × Flexibility + 1.96.
3.4 | Moderation analyses
Variables particularly useful to predict low-verbal ToM scale scores according to stepwise multiple regression
analyses results were CS and WM. We, thus, evaluated the moderation effect of age on the links between low-
verbal ToM scale and these two variables.
Age and CS were used to predict ToM scale scores. The overall model of age and CS were significant predictors
of the low-verbal ToM scale (F(3, 122) = 44.16, p < .001, adjusted R
2
= 0.52). Older children obtained significantly
better low-verbal ToM scale scores (b = 0.35, t(122) = 6.40, p < .001). Low-verbal ToM scale scores also improved
with better mastery of CS (b = 0.10, t(122) = 2.63, p = .01) but they were not significantly predicted by the inter-
action between CS and age (b = −0.002, t(122) = −0.09, p = .93).
Age and WM were used to predict low-verbal ToM scale scores. The overall model of age and WM were sig-
nificant predictors of low-verbal ToM scale (F(3, 111) = 37.04, p < .001, adjusted R
2
= 0.50). Low-verbal ToM scale
scores increased significantly with age (b = 0.40, t(111) = 8.23, p < .001) and with WM levels (b = 0.02, t(111) =
2.93, p = .004) but were not significantly predicted by the interaction between WM and age (b = 0.004, t(111) =
1.44, p = .15).
For ease of comparison with previous literature where ToM was assessed by means of non-verbal FB tasks, we
also evaluated the moderation effect of age on the links between non-verbal FB and CS or WM. Age and CS were
used to predict non-verbal FB scores. The overall model of age and CS were significant predictors of non-verbal FB
(F(3, 112) = 10.35, p < .001, adjusted R
2
= 0.22). Older children obtained significantly better non-verbal FB scores
(b = 0.25, t(112) = 2.44, p = 0.02). Non-verbal FB scores also improved with better mastery of CS (b = 0.16, t(112)
= 2.22, p = 0.03) but they were not significantly predicted by the interaction between CS and age (b = 0.05, t(112)
= 1.69, p = 0.09).
Age and WM were used to predict non-verbal FB scores. The overall model of age and WM were significant
predictors of non-verbal FB (F(3, 112) = 9.54, p < .001, adjusted R
2
= 0.20). Non-verbal FB scores increased sig-
nificantly with age (b = 0.33, t(112) = 3.65, p < .001) and with WM levels (b = 0.02, t(112) = 2.07, p = 0.04) but
they were not significantly predicted by the interaction between WM and age (b = 0.004, t(112) = 0.72, p = 0.47).
TABLE 5 Results of stepwise multiple regression analyses for the low-verbal Theory-of-mind (ToM) scale and
the non-verbal False Belief (FB) task
Dependent variable Steps Predictors N F Adjusted R
2
Low-verbal ToM scale 1 Age 117 F(1,115) = 98.5, p < .001 0.46
2 Complement
sentences
F(2,114) = 56.0, p < .001 0.50
3 Working Memory F(3,113) = 39.7, p < .001 0.51
Non-verbal FB task 1 Relative clause
sentences
116 F(1,114) = 28.5, p < .001 0.20
2 Flexibility F(2,113) = 17.0, p < .001 0.23
  
|
 15
MORGANE Et Al.
4 | DISCUSSION
4.1 | Suitability of tasks for the age range studied
The ToM tasks appear well suited to the age range studied. The low-verbal ToM scale had a 75th percentile below
the maximum score. More specifically, all 3- to 5-year-old children, three quarters of 6-8 year olds and half of
9‒11 year olds performed below ceiling. Regarding the FB task, only a few children performed at ceiling (i.e., 9 out
of 126) with half of children performing above chance level.
Regarding the syntactic tasks, a quarter of the children were at ceiling for the CS and RCS tasks. More spe-
cifically, 25% of 6-8 year olds and 50% of 9-11 year olds obtained maximum scores on the CS task, as did 25% of
9‒11 year olds on the RCS. The syntactic tasks thus appear more prone to ceiling effects.
EF tasks were not at ceiling, with 12% of the entire sample obtaining maximum scores on the inhibition task,
and no child obtaining the maximum score on either the working memory task or the flexibility task. The set of
tasks used thus appear to be well suited to the age range studied, except for the CS task for which a quarter of
the children were at ceiling.
4.2 | Ranking contributions of syntax and EF to ToM
The main goal of the current study was to examine the relative predictive value of syntactic and executive abilities
for ToM.
The correlations indicated that as children grow older, their ToM, syntactic, and executive skills improve. In
addition, results of partial correlations controlling for age were still significant (except for inhibition) with low
intensity for the ToM scale and a very low intensity for the set of FB tasks. These results indicate that apart from
inhibition, other EF skills as well as syntactic skills continue to relate to ToM throughout childhood. This last result,
as well as the absence of a significant correlation between non-verbal FB and inhibition after controlling for the
effect of age, seems contrary to reports that suggest that inhibition still contributes to verbal FB after controlling
for age and vocabulary (Carlson & Moses, 2001; Carlson, Moses, & Breton, 2002; Carlson, Mandell, & Williams,
2004; Chasiotis, Kiessling, Hofer, & Campos, 2006) and could suggest that verbal FB, but not non-verbal FB, is
what is continuously related to inhibition.
Correlation analyses overall suggest that even when verbal and executive demands are kept low in ToM tasks,
the ability to attribute mental states to others in order to predict or explain their behavior improves independent
of age with the mastery of CS with communication verbs, RCS, flexibility and WM. More specifically, a stepwise
multiple regression analysis indicated that among the different syntactic and executive measures, age, CS, and
WM were, in this order, useful to predict ToM scale performance.
Overall, syntactic abilities (i.e., CS with communication verbs) thus appear more essential than executive abili-
ties (i.e., WM) to explain variations in ToM skills. Among syntactic abilities, CS were particularly related to general
ToM abilities. This result was found for low-verbal ToM tasks (Burnel et al., 2018) demonstrating a fundamen-
tal link between ToM and CS understanding (i.e., weak Expression account). In addition, sentences with a similar
syntactic complexity (i.e., RCS) contributed less than CS to ToM scale performance indicating that the semantic
properties of CS allowing misrepresentation, rather than their syntactic complexity, are essential to ToM. It is
worth noting that the ToM tasks administered generally involved children attributing mental states different from
their own (e.g., different desire or belief), which do not necessarily entail a conflict with reality. Moreover, the
specific impact of CS on these tasks was observed for CS with communication verbs and true complements. These
findings suggest that the mastery of syntactic structures allowing for misrepresentation is related to general ToM
abilities, not only the semantics of CS (i.e., mental state verbs) or misrepresentation itself (i.e., false complements).
However, future work should include additional lexical and syntactic measures to control for the potential impact
16 
|
  
MORGANE Et Al.
of general language, and determine whether it may be a more important contributor to ToM than CS. Indeed, a
recent meta-analysis on 18 studies (Farrar et al., 2017) suggests that general language could be predominant for
FB development in typically developing children, while a general language measure was not included in the current
study.
Among executive abilities, WM was found as sufficient, with CS understanding, to predict ToM scale scores.
It is interesting to note that for each of the ToM tasks used in the current study, the WM demand was kept very
low by having pictures depicting the short narrations remain visible throughout the task. Thus, WM appears im-
portant to ToM beyond task demands, which supports the weak Expression account. Specifically, in the current
study we assessed the phonological stock of WM and not the ability to refresh or manipulate information per se.
What appeared the most crucial between the different EF abilities assessed is thus the ability to store short-term
information. Nevertheless, further studies are needed to determine if the link observed here is only due to the
phonological stock size or rather to its link with the ability to refresh or manipulate information.
4.3 | Are FB tasks representative of ToM?
The second goal was to assess whether results previously obtained for FB tasks are representative of results ob-
tained for a broader set of ToM tasks. Results of correlations were similar for non-verbal FB and low-verbal ToM
tasks. Indeed, they were both significantly correlated with age. Moreover, both scores on the ToM scale and on
non-verbal FB task were significantly correlated to CS, RCS, flexibility, and WM but not to inhibition after control-
ling for the effect of age. Some results are, therefore, the same regardless of the ToM measurement used. In addi-
tion, non-verbal FB as well as low-verbal ToM scores were predicted by a combination of syntactic and executive
abilities with a predominant role of syntax over EF (for a similar result with verbal FB tasks, see Durrleman and
Franck, 2015). Nevertheless, the syntactic and executive predictors of ToM scale and non-verbal FB appear to be
very different. We saw that general ToM abilities were better predicted from age, CS, and WM. However, in the
same group of children, non-verbal FB abilities were predicted from RCS and flexibility.
This inconsistency of results for non-verbal FB and low-verbal ToM scale raises several questions. Firstly, CS
appeared to be the major predictor for ToM scale scores but not for non-verbal FB scores. The predominant role
of CS for FB seems to be a classic result in the literature. It is, thus, intriguing that RCS appeared to be more related
to FB compared to CS. Nevertheless, recall that RCS have rarely been directly compared to CS. To our knowledge,
only Hale and Tager-Flusberg (2003) compared the role of CS and RCS training on FB skills. The authors found that
CS but not RCS training improved children’s verbal FB abilities on post-tests. However, in Hale and Tager-Flusberg
(2003), RCS understanding did not improve as much as CS understanding. RCS could, therefore, have required
more training so as to attain the levels required to improve FB abilities. At the same time, the fact that previous
studies assessed false complements alongside verbal FB tasks could have increased the risk of their identifying
links stemming from experimental confounds (cf. Expression account). It is nevertheless theoretically unexpected
that non-verbal FB is better predicted by RCS when low-verbal ToM is better predicted by CS. This could suggest
that syntactic complexity rather than misrepresentation is fundamentally related to non-verbal FB abilities, as
suggested by Smith et al. (2003) as well as Durrleman et al (2018) for verbal FB abilities. Another explanation could
be that a lack of variability in CS scores, due to a ceiling effect, could have prevented us from detecting a relation
to FB scores. However, this is unlikely because CS successfully explained scores on low-verbal ToM scale. This
result requires replication as well as further investigation.
A second inconsistent result is that flexibility appeared particularly important to predict FB abilities, whereas
WM was particularly important to predict ToM abilities. Because the WM demand was kept very low in the ToM
scale used (Burnel et al., 2018), it is unlikely that this task entailed an increased WM demand compared to the
non-verbal FB task. We rather argue that the non-verbal FB task could have entailed an increased flexibility de-
mand. Indeed, this task was part of a larger task including four other conditions (i.e., true belief and two control
  
|
 17
MORGANE Et Al.
conditions) with a randomized order of presentation. This could have increased the requirement of flexibility
because children had to switch between focusing on characters’ mental states and focusing on the physical state
of objects. So, the increased flexibility demand in the FB task could explain why flexibility appeared particularly
useful to predict non-verbal FB abilities. WM was significantly correlated with non-verbal FB, but it did not add
any explanatory power to the model that already included RCS and flexibility. This result thus highlights the im-
portance to use ToM tasks that entail minimal executive demands.
Overall, even though non-verbal FB and low-verbal ToM abilities are predicted by a combination of syntactic
and executive abilities with a predominant role of syntax over EF, the results of the current study indicate that
they do not require the same syntactic and executive abilities. Therefore, FB tasks may not be representative of
general ToM abilities. These results, thus, highlight the need to study ToM beyond FB tasks. However, a limitation
of the current study is that even though we aimed to assess broad ToM abilities, our ToM measure nevertheless
relied solely on five tasks. The results of the current study should thus be replicated using different sets of ToM
tasks.
4.4 | Reasoning and emergence accounts
After identifying the syntactic (i.e., CS) and executive abilities (i.e., WM) particularly useful to predict low-verbal
ToM abilities, our third goal was to assess whether these abilities were important for ToM reasoning per se (i.e.,
Reasoning account) or for ToM development (i.e., Emergence accounts).
As previously mentioned, although extremely useful, the literature on adults does not bring direct evidence in
favor of the Emergence accounts and does not distinguish between the weak Emergence and Reasoning accounts.
By studying children aged between 3 and 11 years old, our goal was to overcome these limitations. We hypoth-
esized that if syntactic and executive abilities are solely useful for ToM development then their relation should
decrease during development. Results of moderation analyses showed that the links between a low-verbal ToM
scale and CS or WM were not significantly moderated by the age of children. Moreover, the same results were
found regarding non-verbal FB abilities. Our results thus do not support the Emergence accounts neither regarding
the ToM-CS links nor regarding the ToM-WM links.
Previous studies showed that adults with WM impairments also exhibit ToM impairments (Rowe, Bullock,
Polkey, & Morris, 2001; Stone, Baron-Cohen, Calder, Keane, & Young, 2003). Thus, the existing literature on
ToM-WM links in adults is in favor of the Reasoning and weak Emergence accounts. Our results, which showed
no evolution of the ToM-WM links between 3 and 11 years of age do not argue in favor of the weak Emergence
hypothesis. They could rather indirectly suggest that the ToM-WM links are constant during development (i.e.,
Reasoning account). Nevertheless, in Null Hypothesis Significance Testing conclusions cannot rely on statistically
non-significant results. Further studies should thus continue to explore the ToM-WM relation during develop-
ment, especially between 11 years old and adulthood.
Overall, studies on the ToM-syntax links in adulthood are in favor of the strong Emergence account, suggest-
ing that language, and more specifically embedded syntax such as CS, is not clearly implied in ToM at adult age.
Nevertheless, in the current study the results are not in favor of an Emergence account. They did not show a
significant decrease in the ToM-syntax links in children aged between 3 and 11 years old, and would, therefore,
indirectly argue in favor of the Reasoning account. Different arguments can be put forward to explain the appar-
ent discrepancy between our results and previous literature. Firstly, studies on adults entail some limitations. It
is not clear if what is affected in patients with aphasia after brain-lesions is linguistic performance or linguistic
competence (Caplan, 1992), thus patients could be able to use language to reason about ToM but not to perform
linguistic tasks. Regarding studies on healthy adults, even if Forgeot d’Arc and Ramus (2011) and Burnel et al.
(2017) used substantial samples (i.e., 58 and 50 participants, respectively), their conclusions rely on statistically
non-significant results which are less reliable and informative than statistically significant results. Limitations of
18 
|
  
MORGANE Et Al.
the current study are also that our conclusions are based on statistically non-significant results, and that we may
have failed to show a different impact of language on ToM depending on age due to a limited sample size or to our
choice of statistical tools. Indeed, according to McClelland and Judd (1993), moderator effects are extremely diffi-
cult to detect. Another possibility that could reconcile our results with results from previous studies could be that
the ToM-syntax links progressively decrease over the course of life (i.e., Emergence accounts) but not significantly
before the age of 11. It is possible that in our study children were not old enough yet to exhibit an important loos-
ening of the ToM-CS links. Further investigations are obviously needed to confirm this hypothesis. These should
take an interest in larger age range, including adolescents. The major difficulty will be to identify adapted syntactic
and low-verbal ToM tasks for older children or adolescents.
5 | CONCLUSION
To sum up, the current study showed that both syntax and EF are useful to predict ToM abilities, with a predomi-
nant role of syntax over EF. More specifically, CS and WM appeared particularly related to ToM beyond the strong
Expression account (i.e., with no confounding variables in the ToM task used). The results of the current study also
provided arguments against the idea that FB tasks can be representative of global ToM, and thus highlight the
need to study ToM beyond FB tasks. In addition, the results did not argue in favor of the weak Emergence account
regarding the ToM-WM links. Combined with literature on adults, they rather suggest that WM is useful for ToM
reasoning regardless of age (i.e., Reasoning account). Regarding the ToM and CS links, even though our results did
not argue in favor of the strong Emergence account in children aged between 3 and 11 years, further investigations
including a larger age span are needed to tease apart Reasoning and strong Emergence accounts, given that results
of previous literature in adults have upheld the strong Emergence account. It is possible that the nature of the
tasks used here, that is, testing global ToM rather than FB, may have contributed to the differences between our
findings and those reported in previous studies. Further investigations should thus continue to study sets of ToM
tasks rather than FB tasks only. Moreover, they should study a larger age range including adolescents. Results of
this study, and further research as suggested, appear useful for understanding cognitive disturbances involved in
social cognition disorders. Indeed, it allows to better identify the neurocognitive substrates and the shared impair-
ments associated with ToM deficits (i.e. syntax, WM, and flexibility) in developmental disorders.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We thank Lou Antoine, Hanna Chesnokova, Rose Spillman, and Raphaëlle Bertolini for their assistance in recruit-
ing participants, as well as for collecting and preparing the data reported in this article. We are especially grateful
to Lou Antoine for her additional help in the realization of the statistical analyses and for her special investment
and interest in the realization of this study. We also thank school directors, teachers, parents, and children for
their participation in the study.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
We declare no conflict of interest.
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
The data that support the findings of this study are openly available in Open Science Framework at http://doi.
org/10.17605/ OSF.IO/S54XU
ORCID
Burnel Morgane https://orcid.org/0000-0002-9383-4893
Durrleman Stéphanie https://orcid.org/0000-0003-1883-5703
  
|
 19
MORGANE Et Al.
Reboul Anne https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3976-5273
Carré Arnaud https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9914-5565
Baciu Monica https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6842-1317
Perrone-Bertolotti Marcela https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9981-7042
ENDNOTE
1
Results of Wilcoxon signed-ranks test indicated that participants had significantly lower reaction times for the con-
gruent items (Mdn = 1.25s) compared to the incongruent items (Mdn = 1.64s) (Z = 9.43, p < .001, r = .84), indicating
that this task is efficient as a measure of inhibition in children aged between 3 and 11 years. In addition, children also
gave significantly more correct answers for the congruent items (Mdn = 1.00) compared to the incongruent items
(Mdn = 0.96) (Z = 5.63, p < .001, r = .50).
REFERENCES
Apperly, I. A., Samson, D., & Humphreys, G. W. (2009). Studies of adults can inform accounts of theory-of-mind develop-
ment. Developmental psychology, 45(1), 190. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014098
Apperly, I. A., Samson, D., Carroll, N., Hussain, S., & Humphreys, G. (2006). Intact first-and second-order false be-
lief reasoning in a patient with severely impaired grammar. Social Neuroscience, 1(3–4), 334–348. https://doi.
org/10.1080/17470 91060 1038693
Austin, G., Groppe, K., & Elsner, B. (2014). The reciprocal relationship between executive function and theory-of-mind
in middle childhood: A 1-year longitudinal perspective. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 655. https://doi.org/10.3389/
fpsyg.2014.00655
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory-of-mind”? Cognition, 21(1), 3746.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022 -8
Begeer, S., Wierda, M., Scheeren, A. M., Teunisse, J. P., Koot, H. M., & Geurts, H. M. (2014). Verbal fluency in chil-
dren with autism spectrum disorders: Clustering and switching strategies. Autism, 18(8), 1014–1018. https://doi.
org/10.1177/13623 61313 500381
Bloom, P., & German, T. P. (2000). Two reasons to abandon the false belief task as a test of theory-of-mind. Cognition,
77(1), B25–B31. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010 -0277(00)00096 -2
Brock, L . L., Kim, H., Gutshall, C. C., & Grissmer, D. W. (2018). The development of theory of mind: Predictors and modera-
tors of improvement in kindergarten. Early Child Development and Care, 189(12), 111. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004
430.2017.1423481
Bull, R., Phillips, L. H., & Conway, C. A. (2008). The role of control functions in mentalizing: Dual-task studies of theo-
ry-of-mind and executive function. Cognition, 107(2), 663–672. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogni tion.2007.07.015
Burnel, M. C., Perrone-Bertolotti, M., Durrleman, S., Reboul, A. C., & Baciu, M. V. (2017). Role of two types of syntactic
embedding in belief attribution in adults with or without Asperger syndrome. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 743. https://
doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00743
Burnel, M., Perrone-Bertolotti, M., Baciu, M., Reboul, A., & Durrleman, S. (2018). Reducing the language content in ToM
tests: A developmental scale. Developmental Psychology, 54(2), 293.
Caplan, D. (1992). Language: Structure, processing and disorders. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/
s0142 71640 0007499
Carlson, S. M., & Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory-of-mind. Child
Development, 72(4), 10321053. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00333
Carlson, S. M., Mandell, D. J., & Williams, L. (2004). Executive function and theory-of-mind: stability and prediction from
ages 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 40(6), 1105. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1105
Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Breton, C. (2002). How specific is the relation between executive function and theo-
ry-of-mind? Contributions of inhibitory control and working memory. Infant and Child Development, 11(2), 73–92.
https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.298
Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Claxton, L. J. (2004). Individual differences in executive functioning and theory of mind:
An investigation of inhibitory control and planning ability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87(4), 299–319.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2004.01.002
Chasiotis, A., Kiessling, F., Hofer, J., & Campos, D. (2006). Theory-of-mind and inhibitory control in three cultures:
Conflict inhibition predicts false belief understanding in Germany, Costa Rica and Cameroon. International Journal of
Behavioral Development, 30(3), 249–260. https://doi.org/10.1177/01650 25406 066759
20 
|
  
MORGANE Et Al.
Craven, P. L. (2005). Testing expression effects of language and inhibition within a series of FB tasks (Doctoral dissertation).
The Pennsylvania State University, State College, PA.
de Villiers, J. G., & De Villiers, P. A. (2000). Linguistic determinism and the understanding of false.In P. Mitchell & K. Riggs
(Eds.) Children’s reasoning and the mind (pp. 191228). Hove: Psychology Press.
de Villiers, P. A., & de Villiers, J. G. (2012). Deception dissociates from false belief reasoning in deaf children: Implications
for the implicit versus explicit theory of mind distinction. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30(1), 188–209.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835x.2011.02072.x
de Villiers, J. G., & Pyers, J. E. (2002). Complements to cognition: a longitudinal study of the relationship between com-
plex syntax and false-belief understanding. Cognitive Development, 17, 10371060. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0885
-2014(02)00073 -4
Dennett, D. C. (1978). Beliefs about beliefs [P&W, SR&B]. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(04), 568–570. https://doi.
org/10.1017/s0140 525x0 0076664
Devine, R. T., & Hughes, C. (2014). Relations between false belief understanding and executive function in early child-
hood: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 85(5), 17771794. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12237
Dodell-Feder, D., Koster-Hale, J., Bedny, M., & Saxe, R. (2011). fMRI item analysis in a theory-of-mind task. Neuroimage,
55(2), 705–712. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuro image.2010.12.040
Durrleman, S., Burnel, M., De Villiers, J. G., Thommen, E., Yan, R., & Delage, H. (2019). The impact of grammar on men-
talizing: A training study including children with Autism Spectrum Disorder and Developmental Language Disorder.
Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2478. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02478
Durrleman, S., Burnel, M., Thommen, E., Foudon, N., Sonié, S., Reboul, A., & Fourneret, P. (2016). The language cogni-
tion interface in ASD: Complement sentences and false belief reasoning. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 21,
109120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2015.10.003
Durrleman, S., Hinzen, W., & Franck, J. (2018). False belief and relative clauses in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Journal of
Communication Disorders, 74, 35–44. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomd is.2018.04.001
Durrleman, S., & Franck, J. (2015). Exploring links between language and cognition in autism spectrum disorders:
Complement sentences, false belief, and executive functioning. Journal of Communication Disorders, 54, 15–31.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomd is.2014.12.001
Farrar, M. J., Benigno, J. P., Tompkins, V., & Gage, N. A. (2017). Are there different pathways to explicit false belief under-
standing? General language and complementation in typical and atypical children. Cognitive Development, 43, 49–66.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.02.005
Farrar, M. J., Johnson, B., Tompkins, V., Easters, M., Zilisi-Medus, A., & Benigno, J. P. (2009). Language and theory of mind
in preschool children with specific language impairment. Journal of Communication Disorders, 42(6), 428–441. https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomd is.2009.07.001
Fine, C., Lumsden, J., & Blair, R. J. R. (2001). Dissociation betweentheory-of-mind'and executive functions in a patient
with early left amygdala damage. Brain, 124(2), 287298. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/ 124.2.287
Flynn, E. (2007). The role of inhibitory control in false belief understanding. Infant and Child Development, 16(1), 53–69.
https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.500
Forgeot d’Arc, B. (2009). Belief attribution in children and adults: implicit and explicit measures and links with verbal reasoning
[Doctoral thesis, Paris VI University]. Paris, France. Retrieved from http://www.theses.fr/2009P A066639
Forgeot d'Arc, B., & Ramus, F. (2011). Belief attribution despite verbal interference. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 64(5), 975–990. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.500
Hale, C. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2003). The influence of language on theory-of-mind: A training study. Developmental
Science, 6(3), 346–359. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00289
Henry, J. D., Phillips, L. H., Crawford, J. R., Ietswaart, M., & Summers, F. (2006). Theory-of-mind following traumatic brain
injury: The role of emotion recognition and executive dysfunction. Neuropsychologia, 44(10), 1623–1628. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.neuro psych ologia.2006.03.020
Hughes, C. (1998). Executive function in preschoolers: Links with theory-of-mind and verbal ability. British Journal of
Developmental Psychology, 16(2), 233–253. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835x.1998.tb009 21.x
Hughes, C., & Ensor, R. (2007). Executive function and theory-of-mind: Predictive relations from ages 2 to 4. Developmental
Psychology, 43(6), 1447. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1447
Levrez, C., Bourdin, B., Le Driant, B., dArc, B. F., & Vandromme, L. (2012). The impact of verbal capacity on theory
of mind in deaf and hard of hearing children. American Annals of the Deaf, 157(1), 66–77. https://doi.org/10.1353/
aad.2012.1610
Lewis-Morrarty, E., Dozier, M., Bernard, K., Terracciano, S. M., & Moore, S. V. (2012). Cognitive flexibility and theo-
ry-of-mind outcomes among foster children: Preschool follow-up results of a randomized clinical trial. Journal of
Adolescent Health, 51(2), S17S22. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadoh ealth.2012.05.005
Lohmann, H., & Tomasello, M. (2003). The role of language in the development of false belief understanding: A training
study. Child Development, 74(4), 11301144. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00597
  
|
 21
MORGANE Et Al.
Low, J. (2010). Preschoolers’ implicit and explicit False-Belief understanding: Relations with complex syntactical mastery.
Child Development, 81(2), 597–615. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01418.x
Mahy, C. E., Moses, L. J., & Pfeifer, J. H. (2014). How and where: Theory-of-mind in the brain. Developmental Cognitive
Neuroscience, 9, 68–81. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2014.01.002
Marcovitch, S., O’Brien, M., Calkins, S. D., Leerkes, E. M., Weaver, J. M., & Levine, D. W. (2015). A longitudinal assessment
of the relation between executive function and theory-of-mind at 3, 4, and 5 years. Cognitive Development, 33, 4055.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2014.07.001
McClelland, G. H., & Judd, C. M. (1993). Statistical difficulties of detecting interactions and moderator effects.
Psychological Bulletin, 114(2), 376. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.114.2.376
Miller, C. A. (2001). False belief understanding in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Communication
Disorders, 34(1), 73–86. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0021 -9924(00)00042 -3
Milligan, K., Astington, J. W., & Dack, L. A. (2007). Language and theory-of-mind: meta-analysis of the rela-
tion between language ability and false-belief understanding. Child Development, 78(2), 622–646. https://doi.
org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01018.x
Moses, L. J. (2001). Executive accounts of theory-of-mind development. Child Development, 72(3), 688–690. https://doi.
org/10.1111/1467-8624.00306
Ng, L., Cheung, H., & Xiao, W. (2010). False belief, complementation language, and contextual bias in preschoolers.
International Journal of Behavioral Development, 34(2), 168–179. https://doi.org/10.1177/01650 25409 350954
Osborne, J. W. (2010). Improving your data transformations: Applying the Box-Cox transformation. Practical Assessment,
Research & Evaluation, 15(12), 19.
Pineda-Alhucema, W., Aristizabal, E., Escudero-Cabarcas, J., Acosta-Lopez, J. E., & Vélez, J. I. (2018). Executive function
and theory of mind in children with ADHD: A systematic review. Neuropsychology Review, 28(3), 341–358. https://doi.
org/10.1007/s1106 5-018-9381-9
Poltrock, S. (2011). About the relation between implicit Theory-of-mind & the comprehension of complement sentences
(Doctoral dissertation). Universitätsbibliothek, Potsdam.
Poncelet, M., & Van der Linden, M. (2003). L’évaluation du stock phonologique de la mémoire de travail: élaboration d’une
épreuve de répétition de non-mots pour population francophone. Revue de neuropsychologie, 13(3), 377–407.
Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory-of-mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4),
515–526. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140 525x0 0076512
Rothmayr, C., Sodian, B., Hajak, G., Dohnel, K., Meinhardt, J., & Sommer, M. (2010). Common and distinct neural net-
works for false-belief reasoning and inhibitory control. NeuroImage, 56, 17051713. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuro
image.2010.12.052
Rowe, A. D., Bullock, P. R., Polkey, C. E., & Morris, R. G. (2001). Theory-of-mind'impairment s and their relationship to exec-
utive functioning following frontal lobe excisions. Brain, 124(3), 600–616. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/ 124.3.600
Saxe, R., Carey, S., & Kanwisher, N. (2004). Understanding other minds: linking developmental psychology and functional
neuroimaging. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 87124. https://doi.org/10.1146/annur ev.psych.55.090902.142044
Schick, B., De Villiers, P., De Villiers, J., & Hoffmeister, R. (2007). Language and theory of mind: A study of deaf children.
Child Development, 78(2), 376–396. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01004.x
Sellabona, E. S., Sánchez, C. R., Majoral, E. V., Guitart, M. E., Caballero, F. S., & Ortiz, J. S. (2013). Labelling improves false
belief understanding. A training study. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 16, 114. https://doi.org/10.1017/sjp.2013.8
Siegal, M., Varley, R., & Want, S. C. (2001). Mind over grammar: Reasoning in aphasia and development. Trends in Cognitive
Sciences, 5(7), 296–301. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364 -6613(00)01667 -3
Smith, M., Apperly, I., & White, V. (2003). False belief reasoning and the acquisition of relative clause sentences. Child
Development, 74(6), 17091719. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1467-8624.2003.00633.x
Smith, C. E., & Wu, I. (2016). Mother-child conversations about thoughts, desires, and emotions: Relations to children’s
understanding of the mind. In P. E. Davis-Kean & S. Tang (Eds.), Socializing Children through Language (pp. 49–80).
Cambridge, MA: Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/b978-0-12-80362 4-2.00003 -5
Stone, V. E., Baron-Cohen, S., Calder, A., Keane, J., & Young, A. (2003). Acquired theory-of-mind impairments in individu-
als with bilateral amygdala lesions. Neuropsychologia, 41(2), 209220. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0028 -3932(02)00151
-3
Tager-Flusberg, H., & Joseph, R. M. (2005). How language facilitates the acquisition of false-belief understanding in chil-
dren with autism. In J. W. Astington & J. A. Baird (Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind (pp. 298–318). Oxford,
UK: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acpro f:oso/97801 95159 912.003.0014
Tompkins, V., Farrar, M. J., & Montgomery, D. E. (2019). Speaking your mind: Language and narrative in young children's
theory of mind development. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 56, 109–140. https://doi.org/10.1016/
bs.acdb.2018.11.003
Troyer, A. K., Moscovitch, M., & Winocur, G. (1997). Clustering and switching as two components of verbal fluency: Evidence
from younger and older healthy adults. Neuropsychology, 11(1), 138. https://doi.org/10.1037//0894-4105.11.1.138
22 
|
  
MORGANE Et Al.
Van der Meer, L., Groenewold, N. A., Nolen, W. A., Pijnenborg, M., & Aleman, A. (2011). Inhibit yourself and understand
the other: Neural basis of distinct processes underlying theory-of-mind. NeuroImage, 56, 2364–2374. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.neuro image.2011.03.053
Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks. Child Development, 75(2), 523–541. https://doi.
org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00691.x
Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: the truth about false
belief. Child Development, 72(3), 655–684. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00304
Wilkinson, L. (1999). Task force on statistical inference, American Psychological Association, Science Directorate (1999).
Statistical methods in psychology journals: Guidelines and explanations. American Psychologist, 54(8), 594–604.
Wright, I., Waterman, M., Prescott, H., & Murdoch-Eaton, D. (2003). A new Stroop-like measure of inhibitory function
development: Typical developmental trends. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(4), 561–575. https://doi.
org/10.1111/1469-7610.00145
Yirmiya, N., Erel, O., Shaked, M., & Solomonica-Levi, D. (1998). Meta-analyses comparing theory-of-mind abilities of in-
dividuals with autism, individuals with mental retardation, and normally developing individuals. Psychological Bulletin,
124(3), 283–307. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.124.3.283
How to cite this article: Morgane B, Stéphanie D, Anne R, Arnaud C, Monica B, Marcela P-B. Theory-of-
mind during childhood: Investigating syntactic and executive contributions. Social Development.
2020;00:122. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12471