Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at
http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=hcap20
Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology
ISSN: 1537-4416 (Print) 1537-4424 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hcap20
Sex/Gender Differences in Screening for Autism
Spectrum Disorder: Implications for Evidence-
Based Assessment
Spencer C. Evans, Andrea D. Boan, Catherine Bradley & Laura A. Carpenter
To cite this article: Spencer C. Evans, Andrea D. Boan, Catherine Bradley & Laura A. Carpenter
(2018): Sex/Gender Differences in Screening for Autism Spectrum Disorder: Implications
for Evidence-Based Assessment, Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, DOI:
10.1080/15374416.2018.1437734
To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2018.1437734
Published online: 30 Mar 2018.
Submit your article to this journal
View related articles
View Crossmark data
Sex/Gender Differences in Screening for Autism
Spectrum Disorder: Implications for Evidence-Based
Assessment
Spencer C. Evans
Department of Psychiatry & Behavioral Sciences, Medical University of South Carolina
Andrea D. Boan, Catherine Bradley, and Laura A. Carpenter
Department of Pediatrics, Medical University of South Carolina
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is diagnosed more often in boys than in girls; however, little is
known about the nature of this sex/gender discrepancy or how it relates to diagnostic assessment
practices. This study examined the performance of the Social Communication Q uestionnaire (SCQ)
in screening for ASD among boys and girls. Data were drawn from the South Carolina Childrens
Educational Surveillance Study, a population-based study of ASD prevalence among children
810 years of age. Analyses were conducted using SCQ data from 3,520 children, with direct
assessment data from 272 with elevated SCQ scores. A bifactor model based on the Diagnostic and
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorderss (5th ed.) two ASD symptom domains t the data well and
performed slightly better for girls. In the general population sample, girls exhibited fewer social
communication/interaction and restricted-repetitive behavior symptoms than boys. In the direct
assessment sample, however , girls with ASD showed greater impairment in social communica-
tion/interaction than boys with ASD. Items pertaining to social communication/interaction problems
at ages 45 were among the most diagnostically efcient overall and particularly for girls. Similarly,
receiver operating characteristic analyses suggested that the SCQ performs adequately among boys
and well among girls. Results support the use of the SCQ in screening for ASD but do not indicate
sex/gender-specic cutoffs. Girls with ASD may exhibit pronounced intraindividual decits in
social communication/interaction compared to male peers with ASD and female peers without
ASD. Although more research is needed, careful attention to social communication/interaction
decits around 45 years of age may be especially useful for assessing ASD in girls.
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental
disorder dened by pervasive decits in social communica-
tion and interaction and patterns of restricted, repetitive,
stereotyped behaviors and interests (American Psychiatric
Association [APA], 2013). Beyond these core criteria, how-
ever, there is considerable heterogeneity in the symptom
presentations exhibited by children with ASD, including a
variety of qualitative and quantitative differences (e.g., sever-
ity, language, cognitive ability, co-occurring problems). One
critical factor in understanding ASD symptom variability is
the role of sex/gender
1
(e.g., Goldman, 2013; Lai, Lombardo,
Auyeung, Chakrabarti, & Baron-Cohen, 2015).
Sex/gender differences in ASD are both over- and under-
acknowledged. On one hand, it has been known for decades
that ASD is more common in boys than girls. Estimates
suggest that the true sex/gender ratio is about 3.3 to 1 (based
on higher quality and population-screening studies; Loomes,
Hull, & Mandy, 2017), whereas the ratio among those
Correspondence should be addressed to Spencer C. Evans, Department
of Psychology, Harvard University, 1036 William James Hall, 33 Kirkland
Street, Cambridge, MA 02138. E-mail: [email protected]
Color versions of one or more of the gures in the article can be found
online at www.tandfonline.com/hcap.
1
Following Lai et al. (2015), we use both terms (sex and gender)
together when referring to differences between boys and girls throughout
most of this article to acknowl edge that it is not clear whether bio logical
sex at birth or social gender constructs are the ke y variable; both likely
play a role.
Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 00(00), 115, 2018
Copyright © Society of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology
ISSN: 1537-4416 print/1537-4424 online
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2018.1437734
clinically diagnosed is about 4.5 to 1 (based on administrative
records; Christensen et al., 2016). On the other hand, little is
known about the nature of sex/gender differences in symptom
presentations among children with ASD. To the extent that
ASD presents differently in boys versus girls, it is possible that
girls with ASD are underidentied. Indeed, Loomes et al.s
(2017) nding that the magnitude of the sex/gender discre-
pancy is inversely related to study quality suggests that there is
gender-related ascertainment bias in clinical diagnosis.
Complicating matters further, the evidence base pertaining to
ASDand, by extension, the diagnostic criteria themselves
historically comes from research among overwhelmingly male
samples (e.g., Edwards, Watkins, Lotzadeh, & Poling, 2012;
Watkins, Zimmermann, & Poling, 2014). Thus, although there
is clearly a need for more research to better understand sex/
gender differences in ASD, it is also important to keep in mind
that existing assessment tools and diagnostic criteria may con-
tain sex/gender bias.
The present study seeks to advance the literature on both
of these fronts: (a) to elucidate the extent and nature of sex/
gender differences in ASD symptom presentations using
comprehensive assessment methods and a population-
based sample and (b) to help understand the extent to
which sex/gender bias may be operating in screening for
ASD using the Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ;
Rutter & Bailey, 2003). To investigate these questions, we
analyzed data from a large epidemiological sample of
school-age children who were screened and assessed for
ASD. Thus, the present analysis offers a unique lens for
investigating ASD screening and symptom presentation
among boys and girls with and without the diagnosis.
SEX/GENDER DIFFERENCES IN ASD
Evidence has been mixed with respect to sex/gender differ-
ences in core ASD symptoms. Some studies indicate that boys
have greater social and communicative problems compared
with girls (e.g., Beggiato et al., 2017; Head, McGillivray, &
Stokes, 2014; Hiller, Young, & Weber, 2016), whereas others
show the opposite pattern (e.g., Carter et al., 2007; Frazier,
Georgiades, et al., 2014; Hartley & Sikora, 2009), and still
others show no particular differences in this domain (e.g.,
Bölte, Duketis, Poustka, & Holtmann, 2011;Holtmann,
Bölte, & Poustka, 2007; Mandy, Chilvers, et al., 2012;May,
Cornish, & Rinehart, 2016; Reinhardt, Wetherby,
Schatschneider, & Lord, 2015; Szatmari et al., 2012).
Similarly, some studies have found boys with ASD to exhibit
higher levels of repetitive and stereotyped behaviors than girls
(Beggiato et al., 2017; Bölte et al., 2011; Hartley & Sikora,
2009; May et al., 2016;Szatmarietal.,2012), whereas others
have found no differences in this domain (Carter et al., 2007;
Holtmann et al., 2007; Reinhardt et al., 2015). In their meta-
analysis, Van Wijngaarden-Cremers et al. (2014) found that
among individuals with ASD beyond age 6, males show
higher levels of restricted/repetitive behaviors and interests
than females; no signicant gender differences were found
for social interaction or communication (Van Wijngaarden-
Cremersetal.,20
14). Narrative reviews (e.g., Kirkovski,
Enticott, & Fitzgerald, 2013; Lai et al., 2015) have yielded
similar conclusions.
In addition, there are clinically important sex/gender differ-
ences in ASD that are not related to the core diagnostic symp-
toms. Compared to boys, girls with ASD more often go
undiagno sed or are diagnosed at a later age, particularly girls
with less severe ASD symptoms and more intact language and
cognitive skills (Begeer et al., 2013;Giarellietal.,2010;
Rutherford et al., 2016). Girls with ASD may also be better
able to compensate for symptoms despite having persistent core
decits associated with ASD (Livingston & Happé, 2017),
which might contribute to greater social ca mouage (Hull
et al., 2017). For example, some evidence suggests that girls
with ASD perform better on measures of nonverbal communica-
tion, which may mask their symptoms (R ynkiewicz et al., 2016).
Despite this compensation, research examining peer relation-
ships found that boys and girls with ASD exhibit more simila-
rities with one another than with their same-gender, typically
developing peers; however girls with ASD appear to face more
social, friendship, and language demands than boys with ASD
(Dean et al., 2014). More broadly, girls can exhibit patterns of
restricted interests and repetitive behaviors and social and com-
municative problems which might seem more socially accepta-
ble than the patterns seen in boys with ASD (Lai et al., 2015).
This could help explain why girls with ASD often have more
severe behavioral, emotional, and cognitive problems compared
to boys with ASD (e.g., Frazier, Georg iades, et al., 2014;
Holtmann et al., 2007;Horiuchietal.,2014; Stacy et al.,
2014), and even compared to girls at risk for ASD who are not
ultimately diagnosed (Dworzynski, Ronald, Bolton, & Happé,
2012). That is, perhaps girls must exhibit more severe symp-
toms, impairment, or co-occurring problems to receive a diag-
nosis of ASD.
One possible explanation for these sex/gender differences is
the extreme male brain theory of ASD (Baron-Cohen,
2002). After reviewing the evidence for behavioral sex/gender
differences, Baron-Cohen concluded that on average, males
exhibited weaknesses at empathizing and strengths at system-
atizing compared to females. Thus, ASD could be a disorder of
the extreme male brain, characterized by low levels of
empathizing traits (e.g., social-emotional understanding, prag-
matic language, friendship development and maintenance) and
high levels of systematizing traits (e.g., attention to detail,
preference for rule-based systems and facts, preoccupation
with cause-and-effect systems, and islets of ability; Baron-
Cohen, 2002; Baron-Cohen, Knickmeyer, & Belmonte,
2005). This theory has garnered some support by way of
between-group behavioral differences (e.g., Stauder, Cornet,
& Ponds, 2011; Tan et al., 2015) and evidence linking mascu-
linization and ASD traits to fetal testosterone exposure (e.g.,
Auyeung et al., 2009; Baron-Cohen et al., 2011). However,
2
EVANS, BOAN, BRADLEY, CARPENTER
this account has also been criticized for being too biologically
reductive and neglecting gender socialization processes (e.g.,
Buchen, 2011; Krahn & Fenton, 2012). Some evidence sug-
gests that ASD is a gender-deant disorder rather than a
disorder of masculinization (Bejerot et al., 2012), and other
research suggests that normative sex differences in typically
developing populations are absent in children with ASD (Park
et al., 2012). Further research is needed to clarify these mixed
ndings.
RELEVANCE TO SCREENING
Much of the research just summarized has focused on children
who have already received the diagnosis, sometimes with a non-
ASD comparison group. Although such studies provide insight
into clinical populations, they do relatively little to improve the
assessment of boys and girls whose ASD diagnostic status is
unknown. This is a major gap in the literature. W ithout addres-
sing the nosological and diagnostic challenges pertaining to sex/
gender considerations, any research on ASD based on existing
assessment practices is subject to the underlying problem of not
knowing how ASD should be dened and diagnosed in males
compared to females (Lai et al., 2015). Thus, there is a need for
rigorous population-based assessment research with attention to
sex/gender. It is possible that systematic sex/gender differenc es
could arise at any step in the assessment pipelinefrom elicit-
ing concerns about ASD to the results of diagnostic evaluations.
Screening measures are particularly key for understanding sex/
gender differences in symptom presentation and for addressing
any systematic problems related to which children get referred
for ASD evaluations. Improved interpretation of screening mea-
sures may lead to earlier identicationforchildreninneedof
services. The SCQ (Rutter & Bailey, 2003) is one of the most
widely researched and recommended parent-report screening
measures for ASD in youth (Norris & Lecavalier, 2010;
Ozonoff, Goodlin-Jones, & Solomon, 2005). Although previous
research has investigated the general diagnostic utility of SCQ
and similar measures for screening for ASD (e.g., receiver
operating characteristic [ROC] and sensitivity/specicity;
Barnard-Brak et al., 2016; Chandler et al., 2007; Duvekot,
Van Der Ende, Verhulst, & Greaves-Lord, 2015; Eaves,
W ingert, Ho, & Mickelson, 2006; Ung et al., 2016), there has
been little attention to sex/gender differences. The notable
exception is that some authors have found evidence for little
to no measurement invariance in the SCQ (Wei, Chesnut,
Barnard-Brak, & Richman, 2015) or similar screening measures
(Frazier , Ratliff, et al., 2014; Frazier & Hardan, 2017).
Although the SCQ demonstrates excellent psychometrics
among school-age children (Chesnut, Wei, Barnard-Brak, &
Richman, 2017; Norris & Lecavalier, 2010), its clinical and
research utility is limited by its lack of subscales, yielding
only a single total score. In developing the SCQ (Berument,
Rutter, Lord, Pickles, & Bailey, 1999; Rutter & Bailey,
2003), the authors pulled items from the three Autism
Diagnostic InterviewRevised domains (Lord, Rutter, &
Le Couteur, 1994),
offering one possible subscale structure.
Then they estimated a three-factor exploratory principal
components analysis from their clinical sample of 200,
offering a different possible structure. Neither of these mod-
els has been validated for clinical or research purposes.
Others (Wei et al., 2015) have subsequently adopted the
SCQs exploratory model or developed their own (e.g.,
Gau et al., 2011). However, the most compelling and
copious eviden ce from a variety of ASD measures (e.g.,
Frazier & Hardan, 2017; Frazier, Youngstrom, Kubu,
Sinclair, & Rezai, 2008; Frazier et al., 2012; Mandy,
Charman, et al., 2012) supports the two-domain framework
that was codied in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual
of Mental Disorders (5th ed.; DSM-5;APA,2013). For this
reason, and to optimize the usefulness of our results, we
examine a two-domain bifactor model of the SCQ.
THE PRESENT STUDY
In sum, the literature documents a large sex/gender discre-
pancy in ASD diagnoses and symptoms, with mixed evidence
and explanations as to why. The present study investigates the
extent and nature of sex/gender differences in ASD symptoms
among a large epidemiological sample of school-age children
and how these differences affect the SCQ in screening for
ASD. Specically, we examine (a) the prevalence of ASD
markers in school-age children, overall and by sex/gender;
(b) differences in SCQ results related to sex/gender and ASD
diagnostic status, and their interaction; (c) the diagnostic ef-
ciency of the SCQ in screening for ASD in boys and girls; and
(d) whether different clinical cutoffs should be considered for
boys and girls. Based on previous research, it was hypothe-
sized that, among those with and without ASD diagnoses, boys
would show higher ASD symptoms overall and particularly in
restricted/repetitive interests and behaviors. It was expected
that this would lead to sex/gender-driven measurement pro-
blems, potentially detrimentally affecting the identication of
girls ASD symptoms. Because diagnostic status was used as
our criterion, this study could not examine sex/gender bias in
the diagnostic construct but rather focused on the performance
of the SCQ. Results may help advance assessment practices
and knowledge related to sex/gender differences in ASD or in
the performance of the SCQ.
METHODS
Participants
Data were drawn from the South Carolina Childrens
Educational Surveillance Study (SUCCESS), a population-
SCREENING FOR ASD IN BOYS AND GIRLS 3
based study of ASD prevalence among school-age children.
The study design and methodology has been detailed else-
where (see Carpenter et al., 2016). The present analyses and
descriptive statistics are based on all available data for
children whose parent provided consent and fully completed
the English version of the SCQ (n = 3,520). The target
population consisted of all children born in 2004 living in
a three-county catchment area in coastal South Carolina.
Participants were 8 to 10 years of age at the time of the
initial screening. Those who were invited for a direct assess-
ment were slightly older by the time their evaluation
occurred (M = 10.3 years; SD = 0.5; range = 8.811.4).
Procedures (detailed next) were designed to obtain as
large and representative a sample as possible, and prelimin-
ary results suggest a reason able degree of representativeness
was achieved. In the population-screening sample, racial/
ethnic backgrounds were as follows (roughly similar to
census estimates): 61% non-Hispanic White, 27% non-
Hispanic Black, 6% Hispanic, 3% other, and 3% multiracial.
In the direct assessment sample, racial/ethnic backgro und
proportions were as follows: 44% non-Hispanic White, 37%
non-Hispanic Black, 13% Hispanic, 1% other, and 4% mul-
tiracial. Compared to girls, greater proportions of boys fell
in the clinical range (SCQ 15) and in the at-risk range (8
SCQ < 15; see Table 1) during the screening, rendering
them more likely to be eligible for a direct assessmen t
(35% of boys vs. 24% of girls). Of these, boys (29%)
were more likely to be invited to and ultimately complete
an assessment compared to girls (22%). Thus, the gender
ratio shifted from census-estimated 51% male in the popula-
tion to 49% male in the screening sample to 65% male in
the direct assessment sample. Sociodemographic variables
were not used to adjust the population screening procedures,
direct assessment sampling, clinical assessment, or analyses.
Procedures
All procedures were approved by the researchers institu-
tional review board. As described by Carpenter et al. (2016),
a multiphase sampling design was used. Procedures were
designed to maximize participation rates from the entire
population, including special education students. Extensive
efforts were taken to ensure that the sample was as repre-
sentative as possible, including steps to boost participation
among studen ts from ethnic minority and lower socioeco-
nomic backgrounds. Recruitment and sampling procedures
were developed based on the literature and in partnership
with schools and organizations in the three-county catch-
ment area. Ultimately, 123 of 127 public and private schools
agreed to participate. Within a 2-month period, families of
eligible children received via their school an introductory
letter; packet with cover letter, waiver, and SCQ; and up to
two reminders. Parents were allowed to complete an online
or paper version of the SCQ or to decline. Incentives for
responding were provided for students, parents, and
teachers.
After co mpleting the SCQ, a subset of participants was
identied and invited for an in-person ASD assessment
TABLE 1
Descriptive Statistics for SCQ by Sex/Gender, SCQ Risk Level, and Diagnostic Group
Sex/Gender Comparisons
Full Sample Boys Girls
Population Sample (N, n) n = 3,520 n = 1,731 n = 1,789
SCQ Total Score
M 5.97 6.77 5.19
MSE 0.09 0.14 0.11
SD 5.41 5.91 4.76
Mdn 4.5 5 4
Mode 2 3 1
Range 036 036 034
Skewness 1.71 1.62 1.68
Kurtosis 3.88 3.23 4.03
SCQ Risk Groups (%)
At Risk (SCQ 15) 7.1 9.4 4.9
Subthreshold (8 SCQ 14) 22.4 25.7 19.2
Low Risk (SCQ 7) 70.5 64.9 75.9
Direct Assessment Sample n = 272 n = 177 n =95
SCQ Total Scores by ASD Groups
ASD+, M (SD) 20.98 (6.86) 20.45 (7.03) 24.29 (4.75)
ASD, M (SD) 13.09 (4.66) 13.38 (4.92) 12.65 (4.24)
Distribution of ASD Groups (%)
ASD+ 18.8 24.9 7.4
ASD 81.3 75.1 92.6
Note: SCQ = Social Communication Questionnaire; ASD = Autism Spectrum Disorder.
4 EVANS, BOAN, BRADLEY, CARPENTER
based on their SCQ scores. Given questions regarding the
optimal cutoff value for the SCQ (Eaves et al., 2006; Norris
& Lecavalier, 2010), all those in the at-risk range (SCQ
15; 100% invited; 44% completed; n = 112) and a randomly
selected portion of those in the subthreshold range (8
SCQ 14; 69% invited; 20% completed; n = 160) were
invited for a direct assessment. This included a separa te
informed consent and a comprehensive ASD diagnostic
assessment (measures described next). These direct assess-
ments were completed by doctoral-level psychologists with
appropriate training and expertise in ASD evaluation .
Participants ASD diagnostic status was determined based
on the integration of all assessment data. Examiners were
not blinded to SCQ scores, but neither these nor sex/gender
status were considerations for diagnostic decision making.
All cases were reviewed by the team on a weekly basis, with
diagnostic ambiguity resolved by consensus. Examiners
interrater reliability was 100% for case status.
From the census-estimated population of 8,780 children,
4,185 survey responses were recorded, of which 3,698
(42%) were usable data.
2
The present analyses are based
on data with complete responses on the English SCQ
(excluding Spanish and partial SCQs), resulting in a nal
analytic sample of 3,520 (40% of population), including 272
who ultimately completed a direct assessment.
Measures
Screening
The SCQ Lifetime Form was used to screen for ASD in the
full sample. The SCQ is a brief, standardized checklist of 40
items pertaining to symptoms of ASD, including problems with
communication and reciprocal social interaction, and restricted,
repetitive, and stereotyped behaviors (Rutter & Bailey, 2003).
All items are in a yes/no format; some ask if the child has ever
exhibited the behavior, whereas others focus speci cally on the
period of 45 years of age when symptoms of ASD may
become more apparent. Items assess both atypical and typical
behaviors, the latter being reverse coded. Possible scores range
from 0 to 39, with higher scores indicating greater likelihood of
ASD. To minimize the possibility of parents recognizing ques-
tions as pertaining to ASD symptoms, the project was promoted
as a study of child social development and the SCQ was licensed
by the publisher to be presented as a SUCCESS
Questionnaire; no changes were made to the SCQ instructions
or items. The English Lifetime SCQ has demonstrated ample
evidence of validity and reliability, including good specicity
and sensitivity (Chandler et al., 2007; Chesnut et al., 2017). The
SCQ had good internal consistency in the present study
(Cronbachs α = .82). As previously noted, the SCQ does not
have validated subscales. For the present analyses, three of the
coauthors (two doctoral-level psychologists and one predoctoral
psychology intern) with expertise in ASD evaluation divided the
SCQ items into two subdomains mapping onto DSM-5 ASD
criteria: (a) social communication and interaction (SCI) decits
(25 items; e.g., spontaneously used gestures, smiles back, talks
to be friendly) and (b) restricted and repetitive behavior (RRB;
12 items, e.g., unusual special interests, odd mannerisms, repe-
titive language). Two items pertaining to self-injurious behavior
(SCQ Item 17) and solitary make-believe play (SCQ Item 35),
which are included in the total score, were not included in
subdomain scores because there was no direct correspondence
with DSM-5. This bifactor model was tested and supported via
conrmatory factor analysis (see Results).
Direct Assessment
Consistent with recommendations (e.g., Ozonoff et al.,
2005), multiple instruments and methods were used in the
diagnostic evaluation. First, a structured ASD diagnostic inter-
view was administered to a primary caregiver. This interview
was developed for the SUCCESS study to assess current and
lifetime symptoms of ASD using an integrated set of criteria
that is compatible with DSM-IV (APA, 1994) and DSM-5.
Second, the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, second
edition (ADOS-2; Lord, Luyster, Gotham, & Guthrie, 2012)
was administered. The ADOS-2 is a semistructured, standar-
dized test, commonly considered a gold standard instrument
in ASD assessment. The ADOS-2 facilitates direct observation
of ASD-related behaviors across several developmentally
appropriate tasks and items, yielding a total score representing
the likelihood of ASD and the severity of symptoms. The
ADOS-2 and its predecessors have substantial evidence for
validity, reliability, and utility in assessing ASD (Gotham et al.,
2008;Lordetal.,2012, 2000; Molloy, Murray, Akers,
Mitchell, & Manning-Courtney, 2011). Standard ADOS-2
procedures were followed, with modules determined by the
childs expressive language abilities (96% were Module 3).
Finally, a variety of additional measures were administered
as
sessing broadband (e.g., CBCL/TRF) and narrowband (e.g.,
Social Responsiveness Scale, 2nd ed.) symptoms, adaptive
(Vineland-2) and cognitive functioning (e.g., Kaufman Brief
Intelligence Test, 2nd ed.), language (Childrens
Communication Checklist, 2nd ed.), medical and educational
history, and demographics (see Carpenter et al., 2016). The
DSM-5 ASD diagnoses were determined using clinical best-
estimate procedures incorporating all available data, with pri-
mary consideration to the diagnostic interview and ADOS-2
results (Carpenter et al., 2016).
Analytic Plan
Descriptive statistics of SCQ scores and items were inspected
overall and by sex/gender and diagnostic subgroups. Group
differences in SCQ scores and item endorsements were
2
The other 487 screeners were excluded due to duplicate submissions,
not living in the surveillance area in 2012, declined participation, or
insufcient number of SCQ item responses for scoring.
SCREENING FOR ASD IN BOYS AND GIRLS
5
estimated using t tests (Cohens d effect sizes), analyses of
variance (ANOVAs; partial η
2
effect sizes), and chi-square
tests (Cramérs V effect sizes). Hypothesized SCQ factor
structures were assessed via conrmatory factor analysis
(CFA), with model t evaluated through collective considera-
tion of the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA),
and conrmatory t index (CFI) and TuckerLewis tindex
(TLI). Following recent recommendations (Kline, 2016;Little,
2013), t indices were interpreted collectively as continuous
measures with approximate thresholds (rather than strict cut-
offs) for adequate model t as follows: CFI/TLI .90 and
RMSEA .08. CFAs were estimated in Mplus Version 7
(Muthén & Muthén, 2012) using weighted least squares. All
other analyses were conducted in SPSS Version 24 (IBM,
2016).
The diagnostic utility of SCQ scores and items were
examined through ROC analyses and diagnostic efciency
statistics as follows: sensitivity (proportion of those with
ASD with positive test result
3
out of all those with positive
result), specicity (proportion of those without ASD with
negative test result out of all those with negative result),
positive predictive value (PPV; likelihood of ASD diagnosis
given a positive test result), negative predictive values
(NPV; likelihood of no ASD diagnosis given negative test
result), and diagnostic likelihood ratios (DLRs; calculated as
[sensitivity]/[1 specicity]). Clinically, DLR values repre-
sent the most concise estimate of diagnostic probability.
DLRs around 1 indicate no change in the probability of
the diagnosis, whereas higher DLRs represent increases in
the probability of the diagnosis (e.g., DLRs 2, 5, 10 corre-
spond to 15%, 30%, and 45% increases, respectively), and
DLRs below 1 represent decreasing probability. These esti-
mates should be interpreted according to the pretest and
posttest probabilities of the population being considered
(e.g., the probability of ASD diagnosis in a clinical setting
vs. in the general population) (Youngstrom, 2013). Finally,
ROC analyses were also utilized to consider diagnostic
efciency of the SCQ among boys and girls. Complex
sampling weights were considered but were not used
because they had little inuence on other analyses, suggest-
ing that the data are sufciently representative for none-
pidemiological analyses.
RESULTS
Conrmatory Factor Analysis
The single-factor CFA model (i.e., original SCQ scoring,
with all 39 items loading onto a single construct) t the data
poorly, χ
2
(702) = 17064.53, p < .001, RMSEA = 0.081,
90% condence interval (CI) [0.080, 0.082], CFI = 0.711,
TLI = .695. By contrast, a bifactor model (i.e., with the
single-factor plus subdomain factors of SCI and RRB)
showed acceptable t, χ
2
(665) = 6056.07, p < .001,
RMSEA = 0.048, 90% CI [0.047, 0.049], CFI = 0.905,
TLI = 0.894. The weighted least squaresadjusted Δχ
2
test
was signicant, Δχ
2
(37) = 4305.26, p < .001, conrming
that the bifactor model t the data better than the single-
factor model, and supporting the use of the two subdomain
scores and the total score in subsequent analyses. Factorial
invariance by sex/gender could not be examined due to
nonconvergence of multiple-group models. Thus, the bifac-
tor model was estimated separately by sex/g ender, showing
adequate t for both boys, χ
2
(665) = 3442.52, p < .001,
RMSEA = 0.049, 90% CI [0.048, 0.051], CFI = 0.905,
TLI = 0.894, and girls, χ
2
(665) = 2595.03, p < .001,
RMSEA = 0.040, 90% CI [0.039, 0.042], CFI = 0.924,
TLI = 0.916. This model appears to show better t for
girls than for boys (e.g., nonoverlapping RMSEA CIs).
SCQ Results: Population Screening Sample
Descriptive statistics for SCQ scores are presented in
Table 1 and item endorsement frequencies in Figure 1.As
shown, markers of ASD symptoms were commonly
endorsed in the general population. More than 50% of
boys and girls had four or more SCQ items endorsed, and
about one fourth of the items were endorsed by at least 20%
of the sample. Boys had higher SCQ scores than girls, t
(3518) = 8.77, p < .001, Cohens d = 0.29, and greater
variability in the distribution of scores. A similar pattern
was observed for the SCQ risk subgroups: 35.1% of boys
fell in the elevated ranges (at-risk or subthreshold),
compared to only 24.1% of girls, χ
2
(2,
N = 3,520) = 56.48, p < .001, Cramérs V = 0.13. Boys
also had higher scores than girls for both the RRB domain, t
(3518) = 24.75, p < .001 (boys M = 2.52, SD = 2.83; girls
M = 2.00, SD = 2.58; Cohens d = 0.19) and for the SCI
domain, t(3518) = 57.23, p < .001 (boys M = 3.97,
SD = 4.01; girls M = 3.04, SD = 3.20; Cohens d = 0.26).
Figure 1 presents the frequency of item endorsement by
sex/gender. The most frequently endorsed items overall
included not using gestures at 45 years of age, using odd
or repetitive speech, not spontaneously copying others at
45, getting pronouns mixed up, and making socially inap-
propriate questions or statements. The least frequently
endorsed items included not responding positively to other
children at 45 years of age, not showing things to parents
at 45, not wanting parents to join in her or his enjoyment at
45, showing limited range of facial expressions at 45, and
not able to have a to-and-fro conversation. After adjusting
for multiple comparisons (family-wise Bonferroni approach;
all ps < .0013), about half of the items showed signicantly
pronounced gender differences; in all cases, boys were rated
as having higher symptom counts than girls. As shown in
3
Positive test result = SCQ score falling on or above the cutoff value.
In standard usage, the SCQ cutoff is 15. We adopt this cutoff value as a
default, but also consider alternative cutoff values where specied.
6 EVANS, BOAN, BRADLEY, CARPENTER
Figure 1, items with the greatest sex/gender differences
include interest in parts of toys, unusually intense interests,
not having a best friend, odd mannerisms such as hand
apping, odd/repetitive language, and a variety of social
decits at 45 years of age (e.g., make-believe/imaginative
games, spontaneously copying/joining others).
SCQ Results: Direct Assessment Sample
Among those who completed an in-person diagnostic evalua-
tion, there were signicant differences in the frequencies with
which boys and girls were diagnosed with ASD, χ
2
(1,
N = 272) = 12.41, p < .001, Cramérs V = 0.21, with one
fourth (24.9%) of boys assessed receiving the diagnosis
compared to only 7.4% of girls assessed. A 2 × 2 ANOVA
revealed a signicant difference in SCQ scores between those
with and without a diagnosis of ASD, F(1, 268) = 72.46,
p < .001, partial η
2
= .213. Although there was no main effect
for sex/gender (p = .160) after ASD diagnosis was included
in the model, there was a signicant interaction between sex/
gender and diagnostic status, F(1, 268) = 4.32, p =.039,
partial η
2
= .016. On average, girls diagnosed with ASD had
FIGURE 1 Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder markers in school-age children by sex/gender in the general population. Note:45=45 years of age.
*Signicant difference between boys and girls after family-wise Bonferoni adjustment (ps < .0013).
SCREENING FOR ASD IN BOYS AND GIRLS
7
SCQ scores approximately 4 points higher than boys diag-
nosed with ASD, whereas boys and girls without the diag-
nosis did not differ in their SCQ scores. (See Table 1 for all
descriptive statistics concerning total SCQ scores.)
Next, ANOVAs were reestimated for the RRB and SCI
symptom domains. In the RRB model, there was only a
main effect for ASD diagnosis in the expected direction, F
(1, 268) = 6.22, p = .013, partial η
2
= .023, with diagnosed
children showing higher levels of RRB (M = 7.18,
SD = 3.17) compared to nondiagnosed children (M = 5.17,
SD = 3.11), with no main effect or interaction for sex/gender
(ps > .4). In the SCI model, however, there was a signicant
interaction between diagnostic status and gender, F(1,
268) = 7.67, p = .006, partial η
2
= .028, such that girls
with ASD had SCI scores that were more than 4 point s
higher (M = 16.71, SD = 3.90) than boys with ASD
(M = 12.23, SD = 5.41), whereas the pattern ran in the
opposite direction for nondiagnosed girls (M = 6.93,
SD = 4.17) and boys (M = 7.74, SD = 4.31). There was
also a signicant main effect for diagnostic status, F(1,
268) = 55.57, p < .001, partial η
2
= .172, with those with
ASD showing greater SCI scores than those without. There
was a marginal main effect for gender, F(1, 268) = 3.69,
p = .056, partial η
2
= .014, with boys showing slightly
higher levels of SCI overall (M = 8.86, SD = 4.99) com-
pared to girls (M = 7.65, SD = 4.86).
Diagnostic Efciency and Clinical Cutoffs
The sensitivity, specicity, PPVs, NPVs, and DLRs are
present overall and by sex/gender at the item level in
Table 2 and for the total SCQ scores (various cutoffs) in
Table 3. At the item level, DLRs were relatively higher (> 2)
for items related to spontaneous showing, shari ng, initiation
of joint attention, shared enjoyment, cooperative, imagina-
tive, and spontaneous play, positive social response, not
smiling back, interest in same-age peers, comforting par-
ents, limited range of facial expressions, and odd manner-
isms such as hand apping. Regarding sex/gender
differences, DLRs were higher for girls compared to boys
on items relating to seeking shared enjoyment, pointing,
nodding and shaking yes and no, sharing and show ing,
playing make-believe games, talking to be friendly, having
a to-and-fro conversation, playing cooperative games with
children, not looking when parent spoke, having little inter-
est in same-age peers, and not spontaneously copying
others. Only a few items showed greater DLRs for boys
compared to girls, including odd mannerisms such as hand
apping, and whole-body movem ents (e.g., spinning, boun-
cing). Notably, for both boys and girls the most diagnosti-
cally efcient items were those pertaining to social-
communication/interaction behaviors at 45 years of age.
As shown in Table 3, at the existing clinical cutoff of 15,
the SCQ demonstrated good sensitivity and NPV, moderate
specicity and DLR, and poor PPV. These lower values
were driven by low PPV for both boys and girls (reecting
the high proportion of non-ASD cases above the cutoff) and
low specicity particularly for boys (reecting the high
proportion of ASD cases below the cutoff). Similar patterns
can be seen when alternative cutoffs are considered, with
higher thresholds leading to better specicity, NPVs, and
DLRs and poorer sensitivity and PPVs, and vice versa for
lower thresholds. Figure 2 presents the ROC curves and the
distribution of SCQ scores by sex/gender and diagnostic
status. The SCQ showed better sensitivity and specicity
for the identication of girls with ASD (area under the curve
[AUC] = .977). Still, results showed a good AUC for boys
(.791) and for the overall sample (.824). Al though it is clear
that the SCQ performed differently in boys and girls in this
sample, it is difcult to discern specic cutoffs based on the
observed data due to the truncated range and qualitative
symptom differences as noted above. Stri ctly speaking, the
optimal trade-off between sensitivity and specicity (i.e.,
maximizing the AUC) falls between 15 (boys) and 19
(girls). However, a visual inspection of Figure 2 indicates
that using cutoffs this high would have resulted in 10 boys
(but zero girls) with ASD going unidentied in the directly
assessed sample. To the extent that screening measures
should prioritize sensitivity, these results do not provide
compelling evidence for developing gender-specic cutoffs
or changing the overall clinical cutoff.
DISCUSSION
This study investigated sex/gender differences in ASD
symptoms in a large sample of school-age children assessed
for ASD. By applying rigorous assessment methods to a
population-based sample, this design helps advance the
literature beyond descriptions of sex/gender differences
among diagnosed populations, toward useful clinical and
research recommendations for diagnostic assessment. Our
results coalesce around one interesting conclusion: Unlike
their typically developing peers, girls with ASD have higher
SCQ scores overall, specically greater social communica-
tion problems, compared to boys with ASD. This pattern is
only partially consistent with our hypotheses and may help
explain other aspects of our results, as discussed next.
In the population sample, boys received higher SCQ
scores than girls both overall and in the SCI and RRB
domains, a nding that is roughly co nsistent with prior
research (e.g., Van Wijngaarden-Cremers et al., 2014).
Among those with ASD, however, girls showed higher
SCI
scores than boys. This is consistent with the notion
that girls may need to exhibit more severe difculties to
receive an ASD diagnosisa pattern that has been found on
a variety of variables in previous studies (e.g., Dworzynski
et al., 2012; Frazier, Georgiades, et al., 2014 ; Holtmann
et al., 2007; Horiuchi et al., 2014; Russell, Steer, &
Golding, 2011; Stacy et al., 2014). Our results suggest
8
EVANS, BOAN, BRADLEY, CARPENTER
that, among those likely to be referred for assessment, there
might be little or no sex/gender differences in levels of
RRB, whether overall or in terms of an interaction with
diagnostic status. Thus, despite the robust sex/gender differ-
ences in RRB in the population, these particular behaviors
do not appear to be associated with sex/gender differences
or differentially contribute to an ASD diagnosis for boys
more than girls or vice versa. Over all, these results are in
line with Park et al.s(2012) nding that normative sex/
gender differences may be absent in children with ASD.
Item-level analyses offer further insight into these nd-
ings, with some of the least frequently endorsed items
tending to be most useful for screening. Specically, items
pertaining to childrens spontaneous interaction and socially
TABLE 2
Diagnostic Efciency of SCQ Items for Identifying ASD in Boys and Girls Sorted by Full Sample DLR (Highest to Lowest)
Full Sample Boys Girls
Item
Symptom
Domain Sens Spec PPV NPV DLR Sens Spec PPV NPV DLR Sens Spec PPV NPV DLR
DLR
Diff
33 At 45, Range of Facial Expression SCI .45 .85 .40 .87 2.93 .45 .86 .51 .83 3.18 .43 .83 .17 .95 2.51 0.67
40 At 45, Group Play SCI .76 .73 .39 .93 2.82 .73 .72 .46 .89 2.61 1.00 .74 .23 1.00 3.83 1.22
37 At 45, Response to Peers SCI .59 .79 .39 .89 2.77 .59 .80 .50 .86 3.02 .57 .76 .16 .96 2.39 0.63
30 At 45, Seeking Shared Enjoyment SCI .41 .85 .39 .86 2.76 .36 .83 .41 .80 2.10 .71 .89 .33 .98 6.29 4.19
31 At 45, Offering Comfort SCI .57 .78 .37 .89 2.56 .55 .78 .45 .84 2.50 .71 .77 .20 .97 3.14 0.64
27 At 45, Social Smiling SCI .53 .78 .36 .88 2.39 .52 .76 .42 .83 2.17 .57 .81 .19 .96 2.96 0.79
15 Hand or Finger Mannerisms RRB .55 .76 .35 .88 2.29 .57 .77 .45 .84 2.44 .43 .75 .12 .94 1.71 0.73
29 At 45, Offering to Share SCI .61 .73 .34 .89 2.24 .57 .71 .39 .83 1.94 .86 .76 .22 .99 3.59 1.65
36 At 45, Interest in Peers SCI .69 .67 .33 .90 2.11 .66 .65 .38 .85 1.87 .86 .72 .19 .98 3.02 1.15
34 At 45, Imitative Social Play SCI .75 .64 .32 .92 2.06 .75 .58 .37 .88 1.78 .71 .73 .17 .97 2.62 0.84
28 At 45, Showing and Directing
Attention
SCI .35 .82 .32 .85 2.00 .30 .84 .38 .78 1.87 .71 .80 .22 .97 3.49 1.62
20 At 45, Social Chat SCI .57 .71 .32 .88 1.99 .50 .72 .37 .81 1.80 1.00 .70 .21 1.00 3.38 1.58
26 At 45, Eye Gaze SCI .75 .62 .31 .91 1.98 .73 .62 .39 .87 1.90 .86 .64 .16 .98 2.36 0.46
35 At 45, Imaginative Play .63 .68 .31 .89 1.98 .59 .65 .36 .83 1.67 .86 .74 .21 .98 3.28 1.61
38 At 45, Attention to Voice SCI .57 .70 .31 .88 1.90 .52 .70 .37 .82 1.74 .86 .70 .19 .98 2.90 1.16
17 Self-Injury .33 .82 .30 .84 1.84 .34 .81 .38 .79 1.81 .29 .83 .12 .94 1.68 0.13
16 Complex Body Mannerisms RRB .49 .72 .29 .86 1.78 .52 .74 .40 .83 2.04 .29 .69 .07 .92 .93 1.11
14 Unusual Sensory Interests RRB .61 .66 .29 .88 1.77 .59 .67 .37 .83 1.79 .71 .64 .14 .97 1.96 0.17
39 At 45, Imaginative Play With Peers SCI .73 .59 .29 .90 1.76 .70 .55 .34 .85 1.56 .86 .65 .16 .98 2.43 0.87
18 Unusual Attachment to Objects RRB .45 .74 .28 .85 1.72 .41 .77 .38 .80 1.81 .71 .68 .15 .97 2.24 0.43
24 At 45, Nodding Head Yes SCI .41 .75 .28 .85 1.65 .36 .74 .31 .78 1.38 .71 .77 .20 .97 3.14 1.76
11 Unusual Preoccupations RRB .69 .58 .27 .89 1.63 .68 .56 .34 .84 1.54 .71 .61 .13 .96 1.85 0.31
13
Unusually Intense Interests RRB .82 .48 .27 .92 1.57 .86 .44 .34 .91 1.55 .57 .52 .09 .94 1.20 0.35
21 At 45, Spontaneous Imitation SCI .59 .62 .26 .87 1.53 .57 .56 .30 .80 1.30 .71 .69 .16 .97 2.33 1.03
19 Friends SCI .43 .71 .26 .84 1.49 .41 .69 .31 .78 1.33 .57 .74 .15 .96 2.19 0.86
10 Use of Others Hand SCI .35 .76 .25 .84 1.47 .32 .77 .32 .77 1.41 .57 .74 .15 .96 2.19 0.78
8 Compulsions or Rituals RRB .76 .47 .25 .90 1.44 .80 .46 .33 .87 1.47 .57 .49 .08 .93 1.12 0.35
22 At 45, Pointing to Express Interest SCI .51 .65 .25 .85 1.44 .43 .62 .27 .77 1.13 1.00 .69 .21 1.00 3.26 2.13
2 Conversation SCI .16 .89 .23 .84 1.43 .15 .87 .26 .77 1.18 .20 .92 .13 .95 2.43 1.25
7 Verbal Rituals RRB .68 .52 .22 .89 1.41 .67 .56 .31 .85 1.52 .80 .45 .08 .97 1.45 0.07
25 At 45, Shaking Head No SCI .39 .71 .24 .84 1.38 .34 .68 .26 .76 1.08 .71 .76 .19 .97 2.99 1.91
12 Interest in Parts of Toy RRB .57 .58 .24 .85 1.37 .61 .56 .31 .81 1.38 .29 .63 .06 .92 .76 0.62
6 Idiosyncratic Language RRB .59 .53 .20 .86 1.25 .56 .50 .25 .79 1.13 .80 .56 .10 .98 1.84 0.71
9 Inappropriate Facial Expression SCI .35 .71 .22 .83 1.22 .36 .73 .31 .78 1.34 .29 .68 .07 .92 .90 0.44
3 Stereotyped Utterances RRB .77 .33 .19 .88 1.15 .77 .35 .26 .83 1.18 .80 .29 .06 .96 1.13 0.05
23 At 45, Gestures to Request SCI .63 .41 .20 .83 1.06 .64 .36 .25 .75 1.00 .57 .48 .08 .93 1.09 0.09
4 Inappropriate Questions or
Statements
SCI .59 .40 .17 .83 .99 .59 .43 .24 .78 1.04 .60 .36 .05 .94 .94 0.10
5 Pronoun Reversal RRB .55 .41 .16 .82 .93 .54 .48 .24 .78 1.03 .60 .32 .05 .93 .88 0.15
32 At 45, Quality of Social Overtures SCI .22 .69 .14 .79 .69 .20 .67 .17 .72 .62 .29 .72 .07 .93 1.01 0.39
Note: SCQ = Social Communication Questionnaire; ASD = autism spectrum disorder; DLR = diagnostic likelihood ratio; Sens = sensitivity;
Spec = specicity; PPV = positive predictive value; NPV = negative predictive value; 45=45 years of age; SCI = social communication and interaction;
RRB = restricted and repetitive behavior.
SCREENING FOR ASD IN BOYS AND GIRLS
9
oriented behaviors at ages 45 appear to be among the most
diagnostically efcient (showing good sensitivity and spe-
cicity) for informing a diagnosis of ASD in all children,
and particularly for girls. Persistent SCI decits appear to be
a relatively sensitive and specic marker for differentiating
girls with ASD from their typically developing female
peers. Of interest, hallmark RRB features of ASD were
generally not among the most diagnostically efcient
items; only odd mannerisms such as hand apping showed
good diagnostic efciency for boys and girls.
The bifactor SCQ model t the data better than the
single-factor model, and slightly better for girls than for
boys. Although research examining measurement invariance
is limited, studies have found similar item functioning and
measurement invariance in the SCQ and other screen ing
measures (Frazier & Hardan, 2017; Wei et al., 2015).
Direct comparisons of the SCQ and SRS-2 might be parti-
cularly useful. Previous research suggests that age is a key
factor affecting the performance of ASD symptom scales
and screening measures; different patterns of results have
been found, and other measures might perform better among
children roughly 6 years of age and younger (Barnard-Brak
et al., 2016; Van Wijngaarden-Cremers et al., 2014).
However, in the present sample of 8- to 10-year-old chil-
dren, results supported the SCQs bifactor structure and two-
domain conceptualization of ASD put forth in DSM-5
(APA, 2013; Frazier et al., 2012; Mandy, Charman, et al.,
2012). This suggests that there may be utility in further
validation and clinical/research use of these two subscales
derived from the SCQ. Although the SCQ items have pre-
viously been subdivided according to a variety of different
exploratory (Berument et al., 1999; Gau et al., 2011) and
conceptual/conrmatory (Rutter & Bailey, 2003; Wei et al.,
2015) approaches, these results add to a body of evidence
suggesting that ASD symptoms can be differentiated into a
general domain with two subdomains, with implications for
research and clinical assessment. For example, the SCQ
could be rened not only as a screening tool but also as
secondary instrument to help support or rule out ASD
symptom domains for diagnosis. Due to the different num-
ber of items, however (25 for SCI vs. 12 for RRB), results
should be interpreted with caution, and future work may be
needed to render these scales more comparable.
ROC results show that the SCQ performs adequat ely as a
diagnostic instrument for boys and girls, especially for girls.
These ndings are consistent with the factor analysis, sub-
domain, and item-level sex/gender differences just
described. These results do not provide a compelling reason
to alter the existing cutoff for boys or girls. However,
caution in both directions is warranted: For boys and girls
alike, scores falling in the at-risk range ( 15) are more
likely to be false positives than true positives (probability of
true positive = 43% and 21%, respectively); and among
boys, scores falling below this cutoff (in the subthreshold
range) still had a 10% probability of diagnosis. Thus, the
SCQ should be interpreted cautiously and probabi listically.
Clinically, an SCQ score of 15 or higher is associated with a
small but clinically signicant increase in the probability of
ASD. As scores increase beyond 15, the probability of ASD
increases proportionately, particularly for girls. Scores
between 11 and 15 may increase the probability enough to
warrant careful clinical judgment (e.g., Corsello et al., 2007;
TABLE 3
Diagnostic Efciency Estimates at Varying SCQ Total Score Cutoff Values
Sex/Gender Comparisons
Full Sample Boys Girls
SCQ
Cutoff
Value Sens Spec PPV NPV DLR Sens Spec PPV NPV DLR Sens Spec PPV NPV DLR
9 .98 .14 .21 .97 1.13 .98 .09 .26 .92 1.07 1.00 .21 .09 1.00 1.26
10 .96 .26 .23 .97 1.30 .96 .24 .29 .94 1.26 1.00 .30 .10 1.00 1.42
11 .92 .38 .26 .95 1.49 .91 .37 .32 .92 1.44 1.00 .40 .12 1.00 1.66
12 .84 .48 .27 .93 1.62 .82 .46 .33 .88 1.51 1.00 .51 .14 1.00 2.04
13 .80 .54 .29 .92 1.74 .77 .52 .35 .87 1.61 1.00 .57 .16 1.00 2.31
14 .80 .61 .32 .93 2.07 .77 .61 .40 .89 1.98 1.00 .61 .17 1.00 2.59
15 .80 .68 .37 .94 2.50 .77 .66 .43 .90 2.29 1.00 .71 .21 1.00 3.39
16 .76 .73 .39 .93 2.82 .73 .72 .46 .89 2.62 1.00 .74 .23 1.00 3.83
17 .75 .78 .44 .93 3.43 .71 .77 .51 .89 3.12 1.00 .80 .28 1.00 4.88
18 .73 .81 .46 .93 3.73 .68 .80 .53 .88 3.36 1.00 .82 .30 1.00 5.49
19 .67 .87 .55 .92 5.26 .61 .86 .59 .87 4.29 1.00 .90 .44 1.00 9.80
20 .61 .91 .61 .91 6.72 .57 .90 .66 .86 5.81 .86 .92 .46 .99 1.78
21 .57 .94 .67 .90 8.98 .52 .92 .70 .85 6.95 .86 .95 .60 .99 18.86
Note: Estimates for the standard Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ) cutoff value are in bold. Sens = sensitivity; Spec = specicity; PPV = positive
predictive value; NPV = negative predictive value; DLR = diagnostic likelihood ratio.
10 EVANS, BOAN, BRADLEY, CARPENTER
Eaves et al., 2006). Overall, at the recommended threshold
of 15, the sensitivity and NPV were good, the specicity
and DLR were moderate, and the PPV poor. Thus, for both
boys and girls, a positive value (above the cutoff) does not
necessarily predict a diagnosis of ASD; indeed, a majority
of cases above this cutoff were false-positives. For boys in
particular, ASD cases were common among those in the
subthreshold range. Again, if the full range of possible SCQ
scores were represented in the data, these values might
differ. As a screening instrument within the broader assess-
ment context, false negatives might be more clinically detri-
mental than false positives given that the latter only
indicates the need for further assessment.
We interpret our sex/gender-discrepant ndings as evi-
dence for qualitative rather than quantitative differences in
ASD symptom presentations between boys and girls. The
pattern of sex/gender symptom differences observed among
those with ASD (SCI, boys < girls; RRB, boys = girls) is
qualitatively distinct from the pattern observed in the general
population (SCI, boys > girls; RRB, boys > girls). These
results differ from the conclusions of a recent meta-analysis
by Hull, Mandy, and Petrides (2016), which found no differ-
ence in RRB, and equivocal evidence for social impairment
(Hull et al., 2016). In general, our ndings are broadly con-
sistent with the well-established sex/gender differences in
ASD prevalence (e.g., Christensen et al., 2016;Loomes
et al., 2017) but do not align neatly with existing theories
in the literature that attempt to explain this discrepancy. For
example, if the extreme male brain theory (Baron-Cohen,
2002) were supported, we might expect similar patterns of
malefemale levels of SCI (related to empathizing) and RRB
(related to systematizing) in those with ASD as in those in the
full population sample; this was not the case. Similarly, we
did not nd evidence for a female camouage effect (e.g.,
Hull et al., 2017; Livingston & Happé, 2017; Rynkiewicz
et al., 2016) insofar as parents did not rate girls diagnosed
with ASD as possessing compensatory social skills (i.e.,
lower SCI scores), which might obfuscate their symptoms.
It is possible that camouage may still exist in settings that
parents typically do not actively observe, such as educational
FIGURE 2 Receiver operating characteristic curves and frequency distributions of Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ) scores by sex/gender and
diagnosis. Note. AUC = area under the curve; ASD = Autism Spectrum Disorder.
SCREENING FOR ASD IN BOYS AND GIRLS
11
settings. Notably, the pattern of results shown in Figure 2
suggests it may be more difcult to differentiate ASD versus
non-ASD status in boys than in girls.
Strengths, Limitations, and Implications
One strength of the present study is that comprehensive,
multimethod assessment practices were used, which mini-
mizes the possibility of results being inuenced by bias
or error. In other words, if the present gender-discrepant
results reect assessment error, then it is likely an under-
lying problem in ASD diagnostic criteria and sassessment
tools in general rather than the particulars of the present
study. Herein li es the dilemma arti culate d by Lai et al.
(2015): Because our existing conceptualizations and
instruments (including SCQ, ADOS-2, and DSM-5 cri-
teria) are derived from predominately male ASD samples,
there remains a challenging problem of the chicken and
the egg. That is, to the extent that ASD symptoms truly
manifest differently in boys compared to girls, studies
such as this one are not able to ascertain this difference.
Broader research is needed to understand the qualitative
nature of SCI and RRB among typically and atypically
developing girls.
This does not, however, rule out the possibility of
informant bias affecting SCQ scores, and this should be
considered in interpreting these results. Parents percep-
tions of SCI decits in boys and girls are likely inu-
enced by sex/gender expectations relative to typically
developing same-sex peers. This is consistent with pre-
vious research suggesting that the social relationships
between boys and girls with ASD are more similar than
relationships with their same-gendered peers (Dean et al.,
2014). Sim ilarly, bias ma y be operating in items pertain-
ingto45 years of age, as these rely on parents recall of
behaviors occurring several years ago. The possibility of
response bias might be illustrated in the rates at which
different items were endorsed. Approximately half of
children were rated as not using gestures at 45years
of age, suggesting that parents may be misinterpreting
this item. This may be a l imitat ion of the yes/no format
of the SCQ, which some parents might struggle with. For
example, Frazier et al. (2010) found that 5.1% of unaf-
fected siblings were rated by their caregivers with SCQ
score of 15 or higher. Thus, results of single items should
be interpreted cautiously.
Additional limitations should be noted. First, using SCQ
scores as the basis for direct assessment sampling results in
an articially truncated range and distribution of SCQ
scores, which woul d not be seen if all participants received
all measures. Thus, there may be children with ASD with
scores in the low-risk range (SCQ < 8) who were missed,
whereas those in the at-risk range (SCQ 15) were more
likely to be invited and assessed than those in the subthres-
hold range (8 SCQ 14). Second, despite our large
overall sample size, our direct assessment sample was rela-
tively small in terms of gender-by-diagnosis subgroups,
with 177 boys (only 25% of whom had ASD) and 95 girls
(only 7% of whom had ASD). A related consequence is that
the completion of in-person assessments may have been
higher due to greater levels of parental concern; indeed,
the response rate was higher among the at-risk group
(44%) compared to the subthreshold group (29%).
Although these data are considered to be a representative
sample from a weighted epidemiological study, the present
results should not be generalized to the entire population in
an epidemiological manner (e.g., complex survey weights
were not used in analyses). Rather, these ndings can be
interpreted simply as results of screening and assessment
analyses conducted among the observed data with its limita-
tions as just noted. However, these limitations are also
reective of a larger strength of this studyrepresentative
sampling of a population of more than 8,000 children, with
nearly 50% participation and inclusion of subthreshold chil-
dren so as to not miss more mildly affected cases.
Third, t his study did not use a well-validated, pub-
lished diagnostic interview. Rather, the evaluations used
an unpublished structured diagnostic interview designed
tomapontobothDSM-IV and
DSM-5 criteria,
assessing
lifetime and present symptoms within a reasonable
administration time. Lastly, a larger and more pernicious
problem is the possibility of sex/gender bias in the
diagnosis of ASD itself. The p resent study (and m uch
of ASD research) relies upon diagn ostic criteria that
have dev eloped over the y ears f rom re search largely
among boys with ASD. The present study ut ilized a
rigorous assessment protocol t o ascertain the diagnosi s;
however, to the extent that the ASD construct is gender
biased, the se res ults cann ot she d light on the natu re of
that bias a nd only highlight the ne ed for broader
research.
These ndings have several implications for clinical
and research assessment practices. First, elevated scores
on the SCQ should be taken seriously regardless of sex/
gender. It may be the case that clinically some girls with
ASD are overlooked due to their perceived strengths in
certain domains. These are important questions for a
diagnostic evaluation. During the screening phase, how-
ever, a high score should not be overridden based on
other perceived strengths. In addition, responses to spe-
cic items should be interpreted according to their devel-
opmental and social context. Careful attention might be
given to items addressing social-communication and
interaction behaviors at 45 years of age. In particular,
girls with A SD may exhibit pronounced intraindividual
SCI decits compared to both their male peers with ASD
and their female peers without ASD. Finally, positive
results on screening measures should not be interpreted
as indicating a diagnosis but only a need for a more
comprehensive evaluation.
12
EVANS, BOAN, BRADLEY, CARPENTER
FUNDING
This work was supported by Autism Speaks (7793, 8408)
and the National Institutes of Health Clinical and
Translational Science Award Program (UL1TR001450).
REFERENCES
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical man-
ual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical man-
ual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
Auyeung, B., Baron-Cohen, S., Ashwin, E., Knickmeyer, R., Taylor, K., &
Hackett, G. (2009). Fetal testosterone and autistic traits. British Journal
of Psychology, 100,122. doi:10.1348/000712608X311731
Barnard-Brak, L., Brewer, A., Chesnut, S., Richman, D., & Schaeffer, A.
M. (2016). The sensitivity and specicity of the social communication
questionnaire for autism spectrum with respect to age. Autism Research,
9(8), 838845. doi:10.1002/aur.2016.9.issue-8
Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends
in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 248254. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01904-6
Baron-Cohen, S., Knickmeyer, R. C., & Belmonte, M. K. (2005). Sex
differences in the brain: Implications for explaining autism. Science,
310, 819823. doi:10.1126/science.1115455
Baron-Cohen, S., Lombardo, M. V., Auyeung, B., Ashwin, E., Chakrabarti,
B., & Knickmeyer, R. (2011). Why are autism spectrum conditions more
prevalent in males?. PLoS Biology, 9, e1001081. doi:10.1371/journal.
pbio.1001081
Begeer, S., Mandell, D., Wijnker-Holmes, B., Venderbosch, S., Rem, D.,
Stekelenburg, F., & Koot, H. M. (2013). Sex differences in the timing of
identication among children and adults with autism spectrum disorders.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 11511156.
doi:10.1007/s10803-012-1656-z
Beggiato, A., Peyre, H., Maruani, A., Scheid, I., Rastam, M., Amsellem, F.,
Delorme, R. (2017). Gender differences in autism spectrum disorders:
Divergence among specic core symptoms. Autism Research, 10, 680
689. doi:10.1002/aur.1715
Bejerot, S., Eriksson, J. M., Bonde, S., Carlström, K., Humble, M. B., &
Eriksson, E. (2012).
The extreme male brain revisited: Gender coherence
in adults with autism spectrum disorder. The British Journal of
Psychiatry, 201,116123. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.111.097899
Berument, S. K., Rutter, M., Lord, C., Pickles, A., & Bailey, A. (1999).
Autism screening questionnaire: Diagnostic validity. The British Journal
of Psychiatry, 175, 444451. doi:10.1192/bjp.175.5.444
Bölte, S., Duketis, E., Poustka, F., & Holtmann, M. (2011). Sex differences in
cognitive domains and their clinical correlates in higher-functioning autism
spectrum disorders. Autism, 15,497511. doi:10.1177/1362361310391116
Buchen, L. (2011). Scientists and Autism: When geeks meet. Nature, 479,
2527. doi:10.1038/479025a
Carpenter, L. A., Boan, A. D., Wahlquist, A. E., Cohen, A., Charles, J., Jenner,
W., & Bradley, C. C. (2016). Screening and direct assessment methodology
to determine the prevalence of Autism Spectrum Disorders. Annals of
Epidemiology, 26,395400. doi:10.1016/j.annepidem.2016.04.003
Carter, A. S., Black, D. O., Tewani, S., Connolly, C. E., Kadlec, M. B., &
Tager-Flusberg, H. (2007). Sex differences in toddlers with autism spec-
trum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37,86
97. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0331-7
Chandler, S., Charman, T., Baird, G., Simonoff, E., Loucas, T., Meldrum,
D., Pickles, A. (2007). Validation of the social communication ques-
tionnaire in a population cohort of children with autism spectrum dis-
orders. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent
Psychiatry, 46, 13241332. doi:10.1097/chi.0b013e31812f7d8d
Chesnut, S. R., Wei, T., Barnard-Brak, L., & Richman, D. M.(2017). A
meta-analysis of the social communication questionnaire: Screening for
autism spectrum disorder. Autism, 21(8), 920928.
Christensen, D. L., Baio, J., Braun, K. V., Bilder, D., Charles, J.,
Constantino, J. N., Lee, L. C. (2016). Prevalence and characteristics
of Autism Spectrum Disorder among children aged 8 Years Autism
and
Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 11 Sites, United States,
2012. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report. Surveillance Summaries,
65,123.
Corsello, C., Hus, V., Pickles, A., Risi, S., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., &
Lord, C. (2007). Between a ROC and a hard place: Decision making and
making decisions about using the SCQ. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 48, 932940. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01762.x
Dean, M., Kasari, C., Shih, W., Frankel, F., Whitney, R., Landa, R.,
Harwood, R. (2014). The peer relationships of girls with ASD at school:
Comparison to boys and girls with and without ASD. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 55, 12181225. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12242
Duvekot, J., Van Der Ende, J., Verhulst, F. C., & Greaves-Lord, K. (2015).
The screening accuracy of the parent and teacher-reported social respon-
siveness scale (SRS): Comparison with the 3Di and ADOS. Journal of
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45, 16581672. doi:10.1007/
s10803-014-2323-3
Dwor zynski, K., Ronald, A., Bolton, P., & Happé, F. (2012). How
different are girls and boys above and below the diagnostic threshold
for autism spectru m disorders?. Journal of the American Academy of
Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 51,788797. doi:10.1016/j.
jaac.2012.05.018
Eaves, L. C., Wingert, H. D., Ho, H. H., & Mickelson, E. C. (2006).
Screening for autism spectrum disorders with the Social
Communication Questionnaire. Journal of Developmental and
Behavioral Pediatrics, 27, S95S103. doi:10.1097/00004703-
200604002-00007
Edwards, T. L., Watkins, E. E., Lotzadeh, A. D., & Poling, A. (2012).
Intervention research to benet people with autism: How old are the
participants?. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6, 996999.
doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2011.11.002
Frazier, T. W., Georgiades, S., Bishop, S. L., & Hardan, A. Y. (2014).
Behavioral and cognitive characteristics of females and males with aut-
ism in the Simons Simplex Collection. Journal of the American Academy
of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 53,
329340. doi:10.1016/j.
jaac.2013.12.004
Frazier, T. W., & Hardan, A. Y. (2017). Equivalence of symptom dimen-
sions in females and males with autism. Autism, 21, 749759.
doi:10.1177/1362361316660066
Frazier, T. W., Ratliff, K. R., Gruber, C., Zhang, Y., Law, P. A., &
Constantino, J. N. (2014). Conrmatory factor analytic structure and
measurement invariance of quantitative autistic traits measured by the
Social Responsiveness Scale-2. Autism, 18,3144. doi:10.1177/
1362361313500382
Frazier, T. W., Youngstrom, E. A., Kubu, C. S., Sinclair, L., & Rezai, A.
(2008). Exploratory and conrmatory factor analysis of the autism diag-
nostic interview-revised. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders, 38, 474480. doi:10.1007/s10803-007-0415-z
Frazier, T. W., Youngstrom, E. A., Sinclair, L., Kubu, C. S., Law, P., Rezai,
A., Eng, C. (2010). Autism spectrum disorders as a qualitatively
distinct category from typical behavior in a large, clinically ascertained
sample. Assessment, 17, 308320. doi:10.1177/1073191109356534
Frazier, T. W., Youngstrom, E. A., Speer, L., Embacher, R., Law, P.,
Constantino, J., Eng, C. (2012). Validation of proposed DSM-5
criteria for autism spectrum disorder. Journal of the American Academy
of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 51,2840. doi:10.1016/j.
jaac.2011.09.021
Gau, S. S. F., Lee, C. M., Lai, M. C., Chiu, Y. N., Huang, Y. F., Kao, J. D.,
& Wu , Y. Y. ( 2011). Psychometric properties of the Chinese version of
SCREENING FOR ASD IN BOYS AND GIRLS
13
the Social Communication Questionnaire. Research in Autism Spectrum
Disorders, 5, 809818. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2010.09.010
Giarelli, E., Wiggins, L. D., Rice, C. E., Levy, S. E., Kirby, R. S., Pinto-
Martin, J., & Mandell, D. (2010). Sex differences in the evaluation and
diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders among children. Disability and
Health Journal, 3, 107116. doi:10.1016/j.dhjo.2009.07.001
Goldman, S. (2013). Opinion: Sex, gender and the diagnosis of autismA
biosocial view of the male preponderance. Research in Autism Spectrum
Disorders, 7, 675679. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2013.02.006
Gotham, K., Risi, S., Dawson, G., Tager-Flusberg, H., Joseph, R., Carter,
A., Sigman, M. (2008). A replication of the Autism Diagnostic
Observation Schedule (ADOS) revised algorithms. Journal of the
American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 47, 642651.
doi:10.1097/CHI.0b013e31816bffb7
Hartley, S. L., & Sikora, D. M. (2009). Sex differences in autism spectrum
disorder: An examination of developmental functioning, autistic symp-
toms, and coexisting behavior problems in toddlers. Journal of Autism
and Developmental Disorders, 39, 17151722. doi:10.1007/s10803-009-
0810-8
Head, A. M., McGillivray, J. A., & Stokes, M. A. (2014). Gender differ-
ences in emotionality and sociability in children with autism spectrum
disorders. Molecular Autism, 5,19. doi:10.1186/2040-2392-5-19
Hiller, R. M., Young, R. L., & Weber, N. (2016). Sex differences in pre-
diagnosis concerns for children later diagnosed with autism spectrum
disorder. Autism, 20,7584. doi:10.1177/1362361314568899
Holtmann, M., Bölte, S., & Poustka, F. (2007). Autism spectrum disorders:
Sex differences in autistic behaviour domains and coexisting psycho-
pathology. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 49, 361366.
doi:10.1111/dmcn.2007.49.issue-5
Horiuchi, F., Oka, Y., Uno, H., Kawabe, K., Okada, F., Saito, I., Ueno,
S.
I. (2014). Ageand sexrelated emotional and behavioral problems in
children with autism spectrum disorders: Comparison with control chil-
dren. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 68, 542550. doi:10.1111/
pcn.12164
Hull, L., Mandy, W., & Petrides, K. V. (2016). Behavioural and cognitive
sex/gender differences in autism spectrum condition and typically devel-
oping males and females. Autism, 21, 706727. doi:10.1177/
1362361316669087
Hull, L., Petrides, K. V., Allison, C., Smith, P., Baron-Cohen, S., Lai, M.-C., &
Mandy, W. (2017). Putting on my best normal:Socialcamouaging in
adults with autism spectrum conditions. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 47, 25192534. doi:10.1007/s10803-017-3166-5
IBM. (2016). SPSS Statistics for Windows. In Version 24. Armonk, NY:
Author.
Kirkovski, M., Enticott, P. G., & Fitzgerald, P. B. (2013). A review of the
role of female gender in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism
and Developmental Disorders, 43, 25842603. doi:10.1007/s10803-013-
1811-1
Kline, R. B. (2016). Principles and practice of structural equation model-
ing. New York, NY: Guilford.
Krahn, T. M., & Fenton, A. (2012). The extreme male brain theory of autism
and the potential adverse effects for boys and girls with autism. Journal of
Bioethical Inquiry, 9,93103. doi:10.1007/s11673-011-9350-y
Lai, M. C., Lombardo, M., Auyeung, B., Chakrabarti, B., & Baron-Cohen,
S. (2015). Sex/gender differences and autism: Setting the scene for future
research. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Psychiatry, 54,1124.
Little,
T. D. (2013). Longitudinal structural equation modeling. New York,
NY: Guilford Press.
Livingston, L. A., & Happé, F. (2017). Conceptualising compensation in
neurodevelopmental disorders: Reections from autism spectrum disor-
der. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 80, 729742. doi:10.1016/j.
neubiorev.2017.06.005
Loomes, R., Hull, L., & Mandy, W. P. L. (2017). What is the male-to-
female ratio in autism spectrum disorder? A systematic review and meta-
analysis. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent
Psychiatry, 56, 466474. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2017.03.013
Lord, C., Luyster, R., Gotham, K., & Guthrie, W. (2012). Autism
Diagnostic Observation Schedule, 2nd Edition (ADOS-2) manual.
Torrence, CA: Western Psychological Services.
Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Jr, Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore,
P. C., Rutter,M.(2000). The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule
Generic: A standard measure of social and communication decits
associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 30,205223. doi:10.1023/A:1005592401947
Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism Diagnostic
Interview-Revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for care-
givers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(5), 659-685.
doi:10.1007/BF02172145
Mandy, W., Chilvers, R., Chowdhury, U., Salter, G., Seigal, A., & Skuse,
D. (2012). Sex differences in autism spectrum disorder: Evidence from a
large sample of children and adolescents. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 42, 13041313. doi:10.1007/s10803-011-
1356-0
Mandy, W. P., Charman, T., & Skuse, D. H. (2012). Testing the construct
validity of proposed criteria for DSM-5 autism spectrum disorder.
Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry,
51,4150. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2011.10.013
May, T., Cornish, K., & Rinehart, N. J. (2016). Gender proles of beha-
vioral
attention in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of
Attention Disorders, 20, 627635. doi:10.1177/1087054712455502
Molloy, C. A., Murray, D. S., Akers, R., Mitchell, T., & Manning-Courtney,
P. ( 2011). Use of the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) in
a clinical setting. Autism, 15,143162. doi:10.1177/1362361310379241
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2012). Mplus Users Guide (6th ed.). Los
Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.
Norris, M., & Lecavalier, L.; Screening accuracy of level 2 autism spectrum
disorder rating scales. (2010). Screening Accuracy of Level 2 Autism
Spectrum Disorder Rating Scales. Autism, 14, 263284. doi:10.1177/
1362361309348071
Ozonoff, S., Goodlin-Jones, B. L., & Solomon, M. (2005). Evidence-based
assessment of autism spectrum disorders in children and adolescents.
Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 34, 523540.
doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp3403_8
Park, S., Cho, S. C., Cho, I. H., Kim, B. N., Kim, J. W., Shin, M. S.,
Yoo, H. J. (2012). Sex differences in children with autism spectrum
disorders compared with their unaffected siblings and typically develop-
ing children. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6, 861870.
doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2011.11.006
Reinhardt, V. P., Wetherby, A. M., Schatschneider, C., & Lord, C. (2015).
Examination of Sex Differences in a Large Sample of Young Children
with Autism Spectrum Disorder and Typical Development. Journal of
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45, 697706. doi:10.1007/s10803-
014-2223-6
Russell, G., Steer, C., & Golding, J. (2011). Social and demographic factors
that inuence the diagnosis of autistic spectrum disorders. Social
Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 46, 12831293. doi:10.1007/
s00127-010-0294-z
Rutherford, M., McKenzie, K., Johnson, T., Catchpole, C., OHare, A.,
McClure, I., Murray
, A. (2016). Gender ratio in a clinical population
sample, age of diagnosis and duration of assessment in children and
adults with autism spectrum disorder. Autism, 20, 628634. doi:10.1177/
1362361315617879
Rutter, M., & Bailey, A. (2003). Social Communication Questionnaire.
Torrance, CA: WPS.
14 EVANS, BOAN, BRADLEY, CARPENTER
Ry nkiewicz, A., Schuller, B., Marchi, E., Piana, S., Camurri, A., Lassalle, A., &
Baron-Cohen, S. (2016). An investigation of the female camouage effec tin
autism using a computerized ADOS-2 and a test of sex/gender differences.
Molecular Autism, 7, 10. doi:10.1186/s13229-016-0073-0
Stacy, M. E., Zablotsky, B., Yarger, H. A., Zimmerman, A., Makia, B., &
Lee, L.-C. (2014). Sex differences in co-occurring conditions of children
with autism spectrum disorders. Autism, 18, 965974. doi:10.1177/
1362361313505719
Stauder, J. E. A., Cornet, L. J. M., & Ponds, R. W. H. M. (2011). The
Extreme Male Brain theory and gender role behaviour in persons with an
autism spectrum condition. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5,
12091214. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2011.01.008
Szatmari, P., Liu, X. Q., Goldberg, J., Zwaigenbaum, L., Paterson, A. D.,
WoodburySmith, M., Thompson, A. (2012). Sex differences in
repetitive stereotyped behaviors in autism: Implications for genetic liabi-
lity. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric
Genetics, 159,512. doi:10.1002/ajmg.b.31238
Tan, D. W., Russell-Smith, S. N., Simons, J. M., Maybery, M. T., Leung,
D., Ng, H. L., & Whitehouse, A. J. (2015). Perceived Gender Ratings for
High and Low Scorers on the Autism-Spectrum Quotient Consistent with
the Extreme Male Brain Account of Autism. PloS One, 10, e0131780.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0131780
Ung, D., Johnco, C., McBride, N. M., Howie, F., Scalli, L., & Storch, E. A.
(2016). Optimizing the screening of autism spectrum disorders in
outpatient clinics: An examination of the Social Communication
Questionnaire-Lifetime. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 27,
2128. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2016.03.011
Van Wijngaarden-Cremers, P. J., Van Eeten, E., Groen, W. B., Van
Deurzen, P. A., Oo sterling, I. J., & Van Der Gaag, R. J. (2014).
Gender and age differences in the core triad of impairments in autism
spectrum dis orders: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44,627635. doi:10.1007/
s10803-013-1913-9
Watkins, E. E., Zimmermann, Z. J., & Poling, A. (2014).
The gender of
participants in published research involving people with autism spectrum
disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 8, 143146.
doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2013.10.010
Wei, T., Chesnut, S. R., Barnard-Brak, L., & Richman, D. ( 2015).
Psychometric analysis of the Socia l Communication Questionnaire
using an item-response theory f ramework: Implications for the use of
the life time and current forms. Journal of Psychopathology and
Behavioral Assessment, 37, 469480. doi:10.1007/s10862-014-
9468-4
Youngstrom, E. A. (2013). Future directions in psychological assessment:
combining evidence-based medicine innovations with psychology's his-
torical strengths to enhance utility. Journal of Clinical Child &
Adolescent Psychology, 42(1), 139159. doi:10.1080/
15374416.2012.736358
SCREENING FOR ASD IN BOYS AND GIRLS
15